Distinct visual motion processing impairments in aging and Alzheimer’s disease

Aging and Alzheimer's disease (AD) are associated with declines in the visual perception of self-movement that undermine navigation and independent living. We studied 214 subjects' heading direction and speed discrimination using the radial patterns of visual motion in optic flow. Young (YA), middle-aged (MA), and older normal (ON) subjects, and AD patients viewed optic flow in which we manipulated the motion coherence, spatial texture, and temporal periodicity composition of the visual display. Aging and AD were associated with poorer heading and speed perception at lower temporal periodicity, with smaller effects of spatial texture. AD patients were particularly impaired by motion incoherence created by adding randomly moving dots to the optic flow. We conclude that visual motion processing is impaired by distinct mechanisms in aging and the transition to AD, implying distinct neural mechanisms of impairment.

[1]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.

[2]  S B Steinman,et al.  Topography of the age-related decline in motion sensitivity. , 1995, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[3]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Distinguishing subregions of the human MT+ complex using visual fields and pursuit eye movements. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  M W Greenlee,et al.  Human cortical areas underlying the perception of optic flow: brain imaging studies. , 2000, International review of neurobiology.

[5]  Charles J. Duffy,et al.  Spatial disorientation in Alzheimer’s disease , 2003, Neurology.

[6]  M. Braunstein,et al.  Induced self-motion in central vision. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Cortical networks for motion processing: Effects of focal brain lesions on perception of different motion types , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Kathryn Dickerson,et al.  Distinct mechanisms of impairment in cognitive ageing and Alzheimer's disease. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Spatial summation and its interaction with the temporal integration mechanism in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[10]  G. Trick,et al.  Visual sensitivity to motion , 1991, Neurology.

[11]  A. Watson,et al.  The optimal motion stimulus , 1995, Vision Research.

[12]  Charles J Duffy,et al.  Detecting navigational deficits in cognitive aging and Alzheimer disease using virtual reality , 2008, Neurology.

[13]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Underlying mechanisms of the response specificity of expansion/contraction and rotation cells in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  J. J. Koenderink,et al.  Spatial properties of the visual detectability of moving spatial white noise , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  D. Burr,et al.  Temporal integration of optic flow, measured by contrast and coherence thresholds , 2001, Vision Research.

[16]  G. Andersen,et al.  Necessity of spatial pooling for the perception of heading in nonrigid environments. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  C. Duffy,et al.  Neurophysiological and perceptual correlates of navigational impairment in Alzheimer's disease. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  J. Gibson Optical motions and transformations as stimuli for visual perception. , 1957, Psychological review.

[19]  Christopher Patrick Taylor,et al.  Aging Reduces Center-Surround Antagonism in Visual Motion Processing , 2005, Neuron.

[20]  Charles J Duffy,et al.  Attentional dynamics and visual perception: mechanisms of spatial disorientation in Alzheimer's disease. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual Homing Is Possible Without Landmarks: A Path Integration Study in Virtual Reality , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[22]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Spatio-temporal characteristics of human motion perception , 1993, Vision Research.

[23]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Functional Consequences of an Integration of Motion and Stereopsis in Area MT of Monkey Extrastriate Visual Cortex , 1996, Neural Computation.

[24]  Charles J Duffy,et al.  A visuospatial variant of mild cognitive impairment , 2003, Neurology.

[25]  Robert H. Wurtz,et al.  Multiple temporal components of optic flow responses in MST neurons , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  J. Money,et al.  A standardized road-map test of direction sense , 1965 .

[27]  A Pentland,et al.  Maximum likelihood estimation: The best PEST , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  R. Blake,et al.  Motion Perception Getting Better with Age? , 2005, Neuron.

[29]  L. Mucke,et al.  Spatial cognition and the human navigation network in AD and MCI , 2007, Neurology.

[30]  W. Warren,et al.  The role of central and peripheral vision in perceiving the direction of self-motion , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  Charles J Duffy,et al.  Receptive field dynamics underlying MST neuronal optic flow selectivity. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Seeing motion behind occluders , 1995, Nature.

[33]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. II. Mechanisms of response selectivity revealed by small-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  A. Benton Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual , 1983 .

[35]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[36]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  A. W. Blackwell,et al.  Age differences in perceiving the direction of self-motion from optical flow. , 1989, Journal of gerontology.

[38]  Jie Zhang,et al.  Aging affects the direction selectivity of MT cells in rhesus monkeys , 2010, Neurobiology of Aging.

[39]  R. Snowden Measurement of visual channels by contrast adaptation , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  M. Banks,et al.  Perceiving heading with different retinal regions and types of optic flow , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Broad tuning for spatial frequency of neural mechanisms underlying visual perception of coherent motion , 1994, Nature.

[42]  M Nawrot,et al.  Neural integration of information specifying structure from stereopsis and motion. , 1989, Science.

[43]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[44]  Johannes M. Zanker,et al.  Interaction between primary and secondary mechanisms in human motion perception , 1994, Vision Research.

[45]  Yuanye Ma,et al.  Aging affects the neural representation of speed in Macaque area MT. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  A. M. Derrington,et al.  Slow discrimination of contrast-defined expansion patterns , 2000, Vision Research.

[47]  F. Campbell,et al.  The influence of spatial frequency and contrast on the perception of moving patterns , 1981, Vision Research.

[48]  P. Atchley,et al.  The effect of age, retinal eccentricity, and speed on the detection of optic flow components. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[49]  O E Favreau,et al.  Perceived velocity of moving chromatic gratings. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[50]  K. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Differential aging of motion processing mechanisms: Evidence against general perceptual decline , 2008, Vision Research.

[51]  M. Folstein,et al.  Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS—ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease , 2011, Neurology.

[52]  A. Leventhal,et al.  Degradation of stimulus selectivity of visual cortical cells in senescent rhesus monkeys , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[53]  George J. Andersen,et al.  The discrimination of heading from optic flow is not retinally invariant , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[54]  David Wechsler,et al.  Wechsler Memory scale. , 2005 .

[55]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[56]  R. Snowden,et al.  Motion Perception in the Ageing Visual System: Minimum Motion, Motion Coherence, and Speed Discrimination Thresholds , 2006, Perception.

[57]  C. Duffy MST neurons respond to optic flow and translational movement. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Allison M. McKendrick,et al.  Aging alters surround modulation of perceived contrast. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[59]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Response of monkey MST neurons to optic flow stimuli with shifted centers of motion , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[60]  C. Duffy,et al.  Visual loss and getting lost in Alzheimer’s disease , 1999, Neurology.

[61]  W Makous,et al.  Visual mechanisms of spatial disorientation in Alzheimer's disease. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[62]  G. A. Orban,et al.  Human Brain Regions Involved in Heading Estimation , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  Charles J Duffy,et al.  Approaching objects cause confusion in patients with Alzheimer's disease regarding their direction of self-movement. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[64]  L. O. Harvey,et al.  Efficient estimation of sensory thresholds with ML-PEST. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[65]  J. Robson,et al.  Summation of very close spatial frequencies: the importance of spatial probability summation , 1987, Vision Research.