Unexpected Costs of High Working Memory Capacity Following Directed Forgetting and Contextual Change Manipulations

Greater working memory capacity is usually associated with greater ability to maintain information in the face of interruptions. In two experiments, we found that some types of interruptions actually lead to greater forgetting among high-span people than among low-span people. Specifically, an instruction designed to change mental context resulted in significant forgetting for high-span people but minimal forgetting among the low-span people. Intentional forgetting instructions also resulted in greater forgetting among higher working memory capacity participants than among lower working memory capacity participants. A candidate explanation called the intensified context shift hypothesis is proposed which suggests that high-span people are more context dependent than low-span people.

[1]  R. Engle,et al.  Is working memory capacity task dependent , 1989 .

[2]  Randall W Engle,et al.  Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  Klaus Oberauer,et al.  Working memory capacity and resistance to interference , 2004 .

[4]  C. Brewin,et al.  Thought suppression, intelligence, and working memory capacity. , 2002, Behaviour research and therapy.

[5]  Randall W. Engle,et al.  Working Memory Capacity and Suppression , 1998 .

[6]  P. F. Delaney,et al.  Directed forgetting in incidental learning and recognition testing: support for a two-factor account. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Individual Differences in the Fan Effect and Working Memory Capacity. , 2004 .

[8]  R. Burchfield Frequency Analysis of English Usage: Lexicon and Grammar. By W. Nelson Francis and Henry Kučera with the assistance of Andrew W. Mackie. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 1982. x + 561 , 1985 .

[9]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  R. Engle,et al.  Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: limits on long-term memory retrieval. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  Working memory and retrieval: a resource-dependent inhibition model. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  R. Engle,et al.  Working-memory capacity as long-term memory activation: an individual-differences approach. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Individual differences in working memory and reading , 1980 .

[14]  Randall W. Engle,et al.  A temporal–contextual retrieval account of complex span: An analysis of errors , 2006 .

[15]  Colleen M. Kelley,et al.  Self-evaluation as a moderating factor of strategy change in directed forgetting benefits , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[16]  D. LaBerge,et al.  The modification of short-term memory through instructions to forget , 1968 .

[17]  M. Conway,et al.  Disruption of Inhibitory Control of Memory Following Lesions to the Frontal and Temporal Lobes , 2003, Cortex.

[18]  Jan Noyes,et al.  The disruption and dissolution of directed forgetting : Inhibitory control of memory , 2000 .

[19]  Peter F. Delaney,et al.  Can encoding differences explain the benefits of directed forgetting in the list-method paradigm? , 2003 .

[20]  R. Engle,et al.  The role of working memory capacity in retrieval. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[21]  Steven M. Smith,et al.  Environmental context-dependent memory: A review and meta-analysis , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  A. Milne,et al.  On the regulation of recollection: The intentional forgetting of stereotypical memories , 1997 .

[23]  Daniel M. Wegner,et al.  Paradoxical Effects of Thought Suppression , 1987 .

[24]  Martin E. Knowles,et al.  Encoding strategy changes and spacing effects in the free recall of unmixed lists , 2005 .

[25]  Colleen M. Kelley,et al.  A contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[26]  D M Wegner,et al.  Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  J. Jaccard,et al.  Interaction effects in multiple regression , 1992 .

[28]  J. Raaijmakers,et al.  A model for interference and forgetting , 1988 .