Effects of dopamine depletion from the caudate-putamen and nucleus accumbens septi on the acquisition and performance of a conditional discrimination task

Three experiments compared the effects of dopamine depletion from the caudate-putamen (CAUD; dorsal striatum) or nucleus accumbens septi (NAS; ventral striatum), or a systemically administered dopamine receptor antagonist (alpha-flupenthixol) on the acquisition and performance of a conditional discrimination task involving temporal frequency. In Expt. 1, rats receiving 6-hydroxydopamine (6-OHDA) lesions of the CAUD were impaired in the acquisition of a visual version of the task, and rats with 6-OHDA lesions of the NAS were not reliably impaired. Even when the rats with CAUD lesions had acquired the discrimination, they were still significantly slower to collect earned food pellets. Both CAUD and NAS lesions reduced a bias to respond to the faster of the two discriminative stimuli. In Expt. 2, rats with 6-OHDA lesions of CAUD were markedly impaired in their accuracy and speed of responding when they had been trained to criterion preoperatively. These effects could not be mimicked in controls by prefeeding (which had only minor effects on performance). Rats with 6-OHDA-induced lesions of the NAS were unimpaired in either visual or auditory discrimination performance, but were slower to extinguish responding than controls. In Expt. 3, alpha-flupenthixol (0.1-0.56 mg/kg, i.p.) produced dose-dependent impairments in both latency to respond and choice accuracy in visual and auditory versions of the task. In conjunction with other results, these data suggest that (1) dopamine receptor blockade and central dopamine depletion can impair discrimination performance under certain conditions (2) dopamine depletion from the ventral and dorsal striatum, respectively, have dissociable effects on behaviour controlled by conditioned reinforcers and discriminative stimuli and (3) the disruption of discrimination performance by dorsal striatal dopamine depletion is probably attributable to several factors.

[1]  C. Marsden,et al.  The performance on learning tasks of patients in the early stages of Parkinson's disease , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  T. Robbins,et al.  Depletion of unilateral striatal dopamine impairs initiation of contralateral actions and not sensory attention , 1985, Nature.

[3]  T. Robbins,et al.  Comparative effects of quisqualic and ibotenic acid-induced lesions of the substantia innominata and globus pallidus on the acquisition of a conditional visual discrimination: Differential effects on cholinergic mechanisms , 1989, Neuroscience.

[4]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Dopamine depletion, stimulation or blockade in the rat disrupts spatial navigation and locomotion dependent upon beacon or distal cues , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[5]  T. Robbins,et al.  A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  M. Petrides Motor conditional associative-learning after selective prefrontal lesions in the monkey , 1982, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  C. Marsden,et al.  'Subcortical dementia': the neuropsychological evidence. , 1988, Neuroscience.

[8]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[9]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens septi and olfactory tubercle on feeding, locomotor activity, and amphetamine anorexia in the rat. , 1978, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[10]  R. Morris,et al.  The effects of central catecholamine depletions on spatial learning in rats , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  D. Rogers Bradyphrenia in parkinsonism: a historical review , 1986, Psychological Medicine.

[12]  H. Fibiger,et al.  Discriminated escape learning and response to electric shock after 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nigro-neostriatal dopaminergic projection , 1975, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[13]  M. Le Moal,et al.  Alternation behavior, spatial discrimination, and reversal disturbances following 6-hydroxydopamine lesions in the nucleus accumbens of the rat. , 1985, Behavioral and neural biology.

[14]  U. Ungerstedt Adipsia and aphagia after 6-hydroxydopamine induced degeneration of the nigro-striatal dopamine system. , 1971, Acta physiologica Scandinavica. Supplementum.

[15]  S. Iversen,et al.  Neurotoxic lesions of ventrolateral but not anteromedial neostriatum in rats impair differential reinforcement of low rates (DRL) performance , 1982, Behavioural Brain Research.

[16]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of excitotoxic lesions of the substantia innominata, ventral and dorsal globus pallidus on the acquisition and retention of a conditional visual discrimination: Implications for cholinergic hypotheses of learning and memory , 1987, Neuroscience.

[17]  R. Passingham,et al.  Premotor cortex in the rat. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Behavioral effects of selective ablation of the caudate nucleus. , 1967, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[19]  M. Petrides Deficits on conditional associative-learning tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of excitotoxic lesions of the substantia innominata, ventral and dorsal globus pallidus on visual discrimination acquisition, performance and reversal in the rat , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[21]  S. Iversen,et al.  Amphetamine and apomorphine responses in the rat following 6-OHDA lesions of the nucleus accumbens septi and corpus striatum , 1975, Brain Research.

[22]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Affinity for the dopamine D2 receptor predicts neuroleptic potency in decreasing the speed of an internal clock , 1986, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[23]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of unilateral dorsal and ventral striatal dopamine depletion on visual neglect in the rat: A neural and behavioural analysis , 1989, Neuroscience.

[24]  K. Jellinger,et al.  Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington. Clinical, morphological and neurochemical correlations. , 1973, Journal of the neurological sciences.

[25]  N. White Reward or reinforcement: What's the difference? , 1989, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[26]  C. Marsden,et al.  'Frontal' cognitive function in patients with Parkinson's disease 'on' and 'off' levodopa. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  W. Nauta,et al.  The amygdalostriatal projection in the rat—an anatomical study by anterograde and retrograde tracing methods , 1982, Neuroscience.

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of ibotenic acid lesions of the nucleus accumbens on spatial learning and extinction in the rat , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  J. Evenden,et al.  The effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens and caudate nucleus of rats on feeding in a novel environment , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  T. Robbins,et al.  The effects of lesions to ascending noradrenergic neurons on discrimination learning and performance in the rat , 1983, Neuroscience.

[31]  Trevor W. Robbins,et al.  Effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens septi on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction time task in rats: Implications for theories of selective attention and arousal , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.