Invariant geometric characteristics of spatial arm motion

This paper examines up to third-order geometric properties of wrist path and the first-order property of wrist trajectory (wrist speed) for spatial pointing movements. Previous studies report conflicting data regarding the time invariance of wrist-path shape, and most analyses are limited to the second-order geometric property (straightness, or strictly speaking, curvature). Subjects performed point-to-point reaching movements between targets whose locations ensured that the wrist paths spanned a range of lengths and lay in various portions of the arm’s spatial workspace. Movement kinematics were recorded using electromagnetic sensors located on the subject’s arm segments and thorax. Analysis revealed that wrist paths tend to lie in planes and to curve more as movement speed decreases. The orientation of the wrist-path plane depends on the reaching task but does not vary significantly with movement speed. The planarity of wrist paths indicates that the paths have close to zero torsion—a third-order geometric property. Wrist-speed profiles showed multiple peaks for sufficiently slow and long lasting movements, indicating deviation from the well-known, bell-shaped profile. These kinematic findings are discussed in light of various motor control theories.

[1]  F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  K. Beck,et al.  Exercise-Induced asthma: diagnosis, treatment, and regulatory issues. , 2002, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[3]  Gerald E. Loeb,et al.  Optimal isn’t good enough , 2012, Biological Cybernetics.

[4]  James P. Schmiedeler,et al.  The leading joint hypothesis for spatial reaching arm motions , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Richard J. Jagacinski,et al.  Quantifying the performance limitations of older and younger adults in a target acquisition task. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  John M. Hollerbach,et al.  Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm movement , 1982, Biological Cybernetics.

[7]  P. Morasso Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[9]  M. Flanders,et al.  Two components of muscle activation: scaling with the speed of arm movement. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Coordination of arm movements in three-dimensional space. Sensorimotor mapping during drawing movement , 1986, Neuroscience.

[11]  Jadran Lenarčič,et al.  Advances in Robot Kinematics , 2000 .

[12]  C. Papaxanthis,et al.  Motor planning of arm movements is direction-dependent in the gravity field , 2007, Neuroscience.

[13]  Howard Poizner,et al.  Influence of movement speed on accuracy and coordination of reaching movements to memorized targets in three-dimensional space in a deafferented subject , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[15]  J. McIntyre,et al.  Kinematic and dynamic processes for the control of pointing movements in humans revealed by short-term exposure to microgravity , 2005, Neuroscience.

[16]  S. Wise,et al.  The Acquisition of Motor Behavior in Vertebrates , 1996 .

[17]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  Satyajit Ambike,et al.  Characteristics of Spatial Human Arm Motion and the Kinematic Trajectory Tracking of Similar Serial Chains , 2011 .

[19]  R. Ivry,et al.  Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence for a common timing mechanism. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  A. G. Feldman Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis (lambda model) for motor control. , 1986, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Moving effortlessly in three dimensions: does Donders' law apply to arm movement? , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  P. Viviani,et al.  Trajectory determines movement dynamics , 1982, Neuroscience.

[23]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[24]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[25]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Learning arm kinematics and dynamics. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[26]  J Richardson,et al.  Spatial patterns in the control of human arm movement. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Vladimir M Zatsiorsky,et al.  Optimization-Based Models of Muscle Coordination , 2002, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[28]  S. Schaal,et al.  Origins and violations of the 2/3 power law in rhythmic three-dimensional arm movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  P. Viviani,et al.  The law relating the kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[30]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  T. Pozzo,et al.  Trajectories of arm pointing movements on the sagittal plane vary with both direction and speed , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Natalia Dounskaia,et al.  Kinematic invariants during cyclical arm movements , 2007, Biological Cybernetics.

[33]  Bryan Buchholz,et al.  ISB recommendation on definitions of joint coordinate systems of various joints for the reporting of human joint motion--Part II: shoulder, elbow, wrist and hand. , 2005, Journal of biomechanics.

[34]  David E. Meyer,et al.  Speed—Accuracy Tradeoffs in Aimed Movements: Toward a Theory of Rapid Voluntary Action , 2018, Attention and Performance XIII.

[35]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Reaction-anticipation transitions in human perception-action patterns , 1996 .

[36]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[37]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  N. C. Krieger Lassen,et al.  On the statistical analysis of orientation data , 1994 .

[39]  Anatol G Feldman,et al.  Space and time in the context of equilibrium-point theory. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[40]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Are arm trajectories planned in kinematic or dynamic coordinates? An adaptation study , 1995, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Central representation of time during motor learning. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[42]  M. Flanders,et al.  Do arm postures vary with the speed of reaching? , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  R G Meulenbroek,et al.  Geometric features of workspace and joint-space paths of 3D reaching movements. , 1998, Acta psychologica.

[44]  Deric Wisleder,et al.  Influence of biomechanical factors on substructure of pointing movements , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  James P. Schmiedeler,et al.  Modeling time invariance in human arm motion coordination , 2006, ARK.

[46]  B. Conrad,et al.  Kinematic properties of slow arm movements in Parkinson's disease , 1994, Journal of Neurology.

[47]  Dagmar Sternad,et al.  Moving the Arm at Different Rates: Slow Movements are Avoided , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[48]  S. Adamovich,et al.  Pointing in 3D space to remembered targets II: Effects of movement speed toward kinesthetically defined targets , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.