Emotion-Associated Tones Attract Enhanced Attention at Early Auditory Processing: Magnetoencephalographic Correlates

Emotionally significant objects and events in our environment attract attention based on their motivational relevance for survival. Such kind of emotional attention is thought to lead to affect-specific amplified processing that closely resembles effects of directed attention. Although there has been extensive research on prioritized processing of visual emotional stimuli, the spatio-temporal dynamics of motivated attention mechanisms in auditory processing are less clearly understood. We investigated modulatory effects of emotional attention at early auditory processing stages using time-sensitive whole-head magnetoencephalography. A novel associative learning procedure involving multiple conditioned stimuli (CSs) per affective category was introduced to specifically test whether affect-specific modulation can proceed in a rapid and highly differentiating fashion in humans. Auditory evoked fields (AEFs) were recorded in response to 42 different ultrashort, click-like sounds before and after affective conditioning with pleasant, unpleasant, or neutral auditory scenes. As hypothesized, emotional attention affected neural click tone processing at time intervals of the P20-50m (20–50 ms) and the N1m (100–130 ms), two early AEF components sensitive to directed selective attention (Woldorff et al., 1993). Distributed source localization revealed amplified processing of tones associated with aversive or pleasant compared with neutral auditory scenes at auditory sensory, frontal and parietal cortex regions. Behavioral tests did not indicate any awareness for the contingent CS–UCS (unconditioned stimulus) relationships in the participants, suggesting affective associative learning in absence of contingency awareness. Our findings imply early and highly differentiating affect-specific modulation of auditory stimulus processing supported by neural mechanisms and circuitry comparable with those reported for directed auditory attention.

[1]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[2]  Richard J Davidson,et al.  Spatio-temporal dynamics of brain mechanisms in aversive classical conditioning: high-density event-related potential and brain electrical tomography analyses , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  O. Luminet,et al.  Aware and (dis)liking: item-based analyses reveal that valence acquisition via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency awareness. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Interpreting magnetic fields of the brain: minimum norm estimates , 2006, Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing.

[5]  Olaf Hauk,et al.  Keep it simple: a case for using classical minimum norm estimation in the analysis of EEG and MEG data , 2004, NeuroImage.

[6]  D. Shanks,et al.  The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[7]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Extinction Learning in Humans Role of the Amygdala and vmPFC , 2004, Neuron.

[8]  Marcia Grabowecky,et al.  Escape from linear time: Prefrontal cortex and conscious experience , 1995 .

[9]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Electrical Signs of Selective Attention in the Human Brain , 1973, Science.

[10]  D. Vaitl,et al.  Neural, electrodermal and behavioral response patterns in contingency aware and unaware subjects during a picture–picture conditioning paradigm , 2009, Neuroscience.

[11]  A. Beck,et al.  An inventory for measuring depression. , 1961, Archives of general psychiatry.

[12]  H Scheich,et al.  Auditory perception of laughing and crying activates human amygdala regardless of attentional state. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  F. Di Russo,et al.  Electrophysiological analysis of cortical mechanisms of selective attention to high and low spatial frequencies , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[14]  Christo Pantev,et al.  Rapid and Highly Resolving: Affective Evaluation of Olfactorily Conditioned Faces , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  C. M. Yee,et al.  Effects of attentional and stressor manipulations on the P50 gating response. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[16]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  A. Keil,et al.  Modulation of the C1 visual event-related component by conditioned stimuli: evidence for sensory plasticity in early affective perception. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Olivier Bertrand,et al.  Dynamics of a Temporo-Fronto-Parietal Network during Sustained Spatial or Spectral Auditory Processing , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Sarah Shomstein,et al.  Parietal Cortex Mediates Voluntary Control of Spatial and Nonspatial Auditory Attention , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Modulation of early auditory processing during selective listening to rapidly presented tones. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Statistical control of artifacts in dense array EEG/MEG studies. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[23]  A. Belger,et al.  Midlatency evoked potentials attenuation and augmentation reflect different aspects of sensory gating , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[24]  Thomas Grunwald,et al.  Neuronal substrates of sensory gating within the human brain , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[25]  Stefan Wiens,et al.  Unawareness is more than a chance event: comment on Lovibond and Shanks (2002). , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[26]  J. Edeline,et al.  Rapid development of learning-induced receptive field plasticity in the auditory cortex. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  Vera Ferrari,et al.  Directed and Motivated Attention during Processing of Natural Scenes , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Motivated attention in emotional picture processing is reflected by activity modulation in cortical attention networks , 2004, NeuroImage.

[29]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[30]  Peter Lang,et al.  Brain processes in emotional perception: Motivated attention , 2004 .

[31]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion, Motivation, and Anxiety: Brain Mechanisms and Psychophysiology the Motivational Organization of Emotion Patterns of Human Emotion Emotion and Perception the Psychophysiology of Picture Processing Neural Imaging: Motivation in the Visual Cortex Motivational Circuits in the Brain , 2022 .

[32]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Brain Systems Mediating Aversive Conditioning: an Event-Related fMRI Study , 1998, Neuron.

[33]  N. Costes,et al.  Emotional Responses to Pleasant and Unpleasant Olfactory, Visual, and Auditory Stimuli: a Positron Emission Tomography Study , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  J. Kissler,et al.  Buzzwords , 2007, Psychological science.

[35]  Robert Freedman,et al.  Gating of auditory evoked responses in normal college students , 1986, Psychiatry Research.

[36]  B. Nagengast,et al.  Evaluative conditioning and the awareness issue: assessing contingency awareness with the four-picture recognition test. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[37]  Henning Scheich,et al.  Learning-induced plasticity in animal and human auditory cortex , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  M. Bradley,et al.  Activation of the visual cortex in motivated attention. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[39]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[40]  O. Lipp,et al.  Does Affective Learning Exist in the Absence of Contingency Awareness , 2001 .

[41]  M. Junghöfer,et al.  Attention and emotion: an ERP analysis of facilitated emotional stimulus processing , 2003, Neuroreport.

[42]  Peter Kirsch,et al.  Hemodynamic responses of the amygdala, the orbitofrontal cortex and the visual cortex during a fear conditioning paradigm. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[43]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala , 1997, Nature.

[44]  Anthony J. Rissling,et al.  Under what conditions can human affective conditioning occur without contingency awareness? Test of the evaluative conditioning paradigm. , 2007, Emotion.

[45]  F. Bloom,et al.  Modulation of early sensory processing in human auditory cortex during auditory selective attention. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  Andreas Keil,et al.  Adaptation in human visual cortex as a mechanism for rapid discrimination of aversive stimuli , 2007, NeuroImage.

[47]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  M. Bradley,et al.  Fleeting images: a new look at early emotion discrimination. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[49]  Markus Junghöfer,et al.  Selective Visual Attention to Emotion , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  Peter J. Lang,et al.  Parallel amygdala and inferotemporal activation reflect emotional intensity and fear relevance , 2005, NeuroImage.

[51]  A. Ioannides,et al.  Attention Modulates Earliest Responses in the Primary Auditory and Visual Cortices , 2008, Neuron.

[52]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal cortex regulates inhibition and excitation in distributed neural networks. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[53]  F. Baeyens,et al.  Contingency awareness in evaluative conditioning: A case for unaware affective-evaluative learning , 1990 .

[54]  S. Luck,et al.  Spatio‐temporal dynamics of attention to color: Evidence from human electrophysiology , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[55]  Christo Pantev,et al.  Sound Processing Hierarchy within Human Auditory Cortex , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Magnetoencephalography (MEG) determined temporal modulation of visual and auditory sensory processing in the context of classical conditioning to faces , 2006, NeuroImage.

[57]  D L Woods,et al.  Lesions of frontal cortex diminish the auditory mismatch negativity. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[58]  T R Bashore,et al.  Attentional influence on the P50 component of the auditory event-related brain potential. , 1992, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[59]  I. Hashimoto,et al.  Rapid change of tonotopic maps in the human auditory cortex during pitch discrimination , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[60]  N. Weinberger Specific long-term memory traces in primary auditory cortex , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[61]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Fear conditioning enhances short-latency auditory responses of lateral amygdala neurons: Parallel recordings in the freely behaving rat , 1995, Neuron.

[62]  K. Alho,et al.  Brain potential signs of feature processing during auditory selective attention. , 1991, Neuroreport.

[63]  S. David,et al.  Auditory attention : focusing the searchlight on sound , 2007 .

[64]  P. Lang Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: computer applications , 1980 .