Counting in a time discrimination task in children and adults

The present study investigated in 5- and 8-year-olds, as well as in adults, the effect of verbal counting on temporal discrimination behavior in a generalization task with two duration ranges in order to test the scalar timing property. The results showed that counting improved temporal sensitivity in all age groups, although sensitivity to time remained lower in the younger children. Furthermore, in the 5-year-olds, the temporal generalization behavior conformed well to the scalar property of variance both in the counting and the non-counting condition. However, this conformity to the scalar timing property disappeared when counting was used in the 8-year-olds and the adults. The development of the ability to count time at a constant rhythm is discussed as the major reason for this departure of temporal behavior from the scalar property of variance when counting is employed.

[1]  Peter R. Killeen,et al.  Counting the Minutes , 1992 .

[2]  Jeffrey R Binder,et al.  Neural systems supporting timing and chronometric counting: an FMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  Ana García-Gutiérrez,et al.  Temporal performance in 4-8 year old children. The effect of chronometric information in task execution. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[4]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[5]  Simon Grondin,et al.  When to start explicit counting in a time-intervals discrimination task: A critical point in the timing process of humans. , 1999 .

[6]  W. Meck Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2003 .

[7]  Iris Levin,et al.  Chapter 4 Principles Underlying Time Measurement: The Development of Children's Constraints on Counting Time* , 1989 .

[8]  J H Wearden,et al.  Scalar timing in temporal generalization in humans with longer stimulus durations. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  R. E. Hicks,et al.  Counting eliminates the repetition effect in judgments of temporal duration. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[10]  J. Wearden,et al.  Temporal generalization in 3- to 8-year-old children. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  William M. Petrusic Explicit Counting and Time‐Order Errors in Duration Discrimination , 1984 .

[12]  Gordon D A Brown,et al.  A timing-specific memory distortion effect in young children. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[13]  R. Church A concise introduction to scalar timing theory. , 2003 .

[14]  David J. Getty,et al.  Counting processes in human timing , 1976 .

[15]  J G Fetterman,et al.  A componential analysis of pacemaker-counter timing systems. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  Sylvie Droit-Volet,et al.  Time and number discrimination in a bisection task with a sequence of stimuli: a developmental approach. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[17]  P. Fraisse The psychology of time , 1963 .

[18]  John H. Wearden,et al.  Do humans possess an internal clock with scalar timing properties , 1991 .

[19]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[20]  J. Wearden Human Performance on an Analogue of an Interval Bisection Task , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[21]  P. Killeen,et al.  Optimal timing and the Weber function. , 1987, Psychological review.

[22]  S. Droit-Volet Scalar timing in temporal generalization in children with short and long stimulus durations , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[23]  J H Wearden,et al.  Temporal bisection in children. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[24]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  T. McCormack,et al.  Developmental changes in time estimation: comparing childhood and old age. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[26]  L. Allan The influence of the scalar timing model on human timing research , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[27]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[28]  S. Grondin,et al.  From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  Dan Zakay,et al.  Time and human cognition : a life-span perspective , 1989 .

[30]  J. Wearden,et al.  Temporal Bisection in Humans with Longer Stimulus Durations , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[31]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[32]  Sylvie Droit-Volet,et al.  The Effect of Feedback on Timing in Children and Adults: The Temporal Generalization Task , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[33]  Friedrich Wilkening,et al.  Children's counting strategies for time quantification and integration , 1987 .

[34]  A. R. Gilliland,et al.  Some factors in estimating short time intervals , 1940 .

[35]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[36]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  “One-thousandone … one-thousandtwo …”: Chronometric counting violates the scalar property in interval timing , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .