Distraction and Mind-Wandering Under Load

Attention research over the last several decades has provided rich insights into the determinants of distraction, including distractor characteristics, task features, and individual differences. Load Theory represented a particularly important breakthrough, highlighting the critical role of the level and nature of task-load in determining both the efficiency of distractor rejection and the stage of processing at which this occurs. However, until recently studies of distraction were restricted to those measuring rather specific forms of distraction by external stimuli which I argue that, although intended to be irrelevant, were in fact task-relevant. In daily life, attention may be distracted by a wide range of stimuli, which may often be entirely unrelated to any task being performed, and may include not only external stimuli but also internally generated stimuli such as task-unrelated thoughts. This review outlines recent research examining these more general, entirely task-irrelevant, forms of distraction within the framework of Load Theory. I discuss the relation between different forms of distraction, and the universality of load effects across different distractor types and individuals.

[1]  N. Lavie,et al.  High Perceptual Load Makes Everybody Equal , 2007, Psychological science.

[2]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[3]  N. Lavie,et al.  The influence of perceptual load on age differences in selective attention. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[4]  N. Lavie Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  N. Lavie,et al.  Harnessing the wandering mind: The role of perceptual load , 2009, Cognition.

[6]  B. Gibson,et al.  Stimulus-driven attentional capture is contingent on attentional set for displaywide visual features. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  N. Lavie Attention, Distraction, and Cognitive Control Under Load , 2010 .

[8]  Nash Unsworth,et al.  Variation in Cognitive Failures: An Individual Differences Investigation of Everyday Attention and Memory Failures. , 2012 .

[9]  Mike Coleman,et al.  Selective Attention and Perceptual Load in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2009, Psychological science.

[10]  P. Silvia,et al.  For Whom the Mind Wanders, and When , 2007, Psychological science.

[11]  Attentional capture by motion onsets is modulated by perceptual load. , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[12]  N. Lavie,et al.  On the Efficiency of Visual Selective Attention: Efficient Visual Search Leads to Inefficient Distractor Rejection , 1997 .

[13]  Y. Tsal,et al.  Diluting the burden of load: perceptual load effects are simply dilution effects. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  N. Lavie Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Dennis Doverspike,et al.  Locus of control and auditory selective attention as predictors of driving accident involvement: A comparative longitudinal investigation☆ , 1992 .

[16]  Jason S. McCarley,et al.  Mind Wandering Behind the Wheel , 2011, Hum. Factors.

[17]  Rob Jenkins,et al.  Neural processing of fearful faces: effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[19]  A. Kramer,et al.  Perceptual organization and focused attention: The role of objects and proximity in visual processing , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[20]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The restless mind. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  Nicola C. Anderson,et al.  Everyday Attention: Variation in Mind Wandering and Memory in a Lecture , 2012 .

[23]  Patrick S. Malone,et al.  An Exploration of the Relationship Between Ethnicity, Attention Problems, and Academic Achievement , 2004 .

[24]  Zach Shipstead,et al.  Working Memory Capacity and Visual Attention: Top-Down and Bottom-Up Guidance , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[25]  R. Dolan,et al.  Attentional load and sensory competition in human vision: modulation of fMRI responses by load at fixation during task-irrelevant stimulation in the peripheral visual field. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  S. Bishop Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[27]  Nilli Lavie,et al.  The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness , 2007, Cognition.

[28]  D. Bavelier,et al.  Changes in the Spatial Distribution of Visual Attention after Early Deafness , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  F. Chua,et al.  Capturing focused attention , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  Daryl E. Wilson,et al.  Dilution, not load, affects distractor processing. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary attentional capture by abrupt onsets , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The lights are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  M. Kane,et al.  Conducting the train of thought: working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[34]  N. Lavie,et al.  Musical expertise modulates the effects of visual perceptual load , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[35]  C. S. Green,et al.  Action video game modifies visual selective attention , 2003, Nature.

[36]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  S. Vecera,et al.  Perceptual load modulates attentional capture by abrupt onsets , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  A. Young,et al.  Face-name interference. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  Shawn E Christ,et al.  Abrupt onsets cannot be ignored , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[40]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  Zoning Out while Reading: Evidence for Dissociations between Experience and Metaconsciousness. , 2004 .

[42]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  The Persistence of Thought , 2012, Psychological science.

[43]  N. Lavie,et al.  The Role of Perceptual Load in Processing Distractor Faces , 2003, Psychological science.

[44]  N. Lavie,et al.  Failures to Ignore Entirely Irrelevant Distractors , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[45]  N Lavie,et al.  The role of perceptual load in negative priming. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  M. Kane,et al.  Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[47]  B. Gibson,et al.  Semantic and affective salience: the role of meaning and preference in attentional capture and disengagement. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  J. Theeuwes Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  R. Engle,et al.  Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  Stephen J. Vodanovich,et al.  Can Accidents and Industrial Mishaps Be Predicted? Further Investigation into the Relationship Between Cognitive Failure and Reports of Accidents , 2003 .

[51]  Nilli Lavie,et al.  Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[52]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[53]  C D Frith,et al.  Modulating irrelevant motion perception by varying attentional load in an unrelated task. , 1997, Science.

[54]  Y. Tsal,et al.  The attentional white bear phenomenon: the mandatory allocation of attention to expected distractor locations. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  S. Yantis 3 Goal-Directed and Stimulus-Driven Determinants of Attentional Control , 2000 .

[56]  Nilli Lavie,et al.  Distracted by your mind? Individual differences in distractibility predict mind wandering. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[57]  N. Lavie,et al.  Attentional capture by entirely irrelevant distractors , 2008 .

[58]  J. Theeuwes Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[59]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[60]  G. Woodman,et al.  Neural fate of ignored stimuli: dissociable effects of perceptual and working memory load , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[61]  T. Carr,et al.  Development of selective attention: perceptual load influences early versus late attentional selection in children and adults. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[62]  G. Baylis,et al.  Movement and visual attention: the spotlight metaphor breaks down. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.