Exergaming: Feels good despite working harder

Strategies to encourage exercise have led to research on cycle ergometer ‘exergaming’, as a means of enhancing exercise enjoyment. This research has typically prescribed the exercise intensity and used one exercise mode. The aim of this study was to compare self-selected exercise intensity on a cycle ergometer with and without exergaming modes activated. A total of 20 participants aged between 18–40 years (M = 24.2 ± 5.9) completed a sub-maximal exercise test. Participants returned two days later to complete one 45 minute session of self-selected exercise with 15 minutes in each of ‘control’ (standard ergometer), ‘track’, and ‘game’ modes, with order randomized. Heart rate, work rate, perceived exertion, and affective valence were recorded during exercise. Dissociation and enjoyment were recorded in the rest interval between each mode. Participants exercised above ventilatory threshold (VT) in all three modes (track, M = 9.5 ± 12%; game, M = 6.2 ± 12%; and control, M = 4.4 ± 14% above VT) and at higher work rates (P < 0.05) exergaming (track, M = 94.5 ± 27.9; game, M = 96.2 ± 32.8 watts) than control (M = 86.6 ± 26.5 watts). Despite exercising at a higher intensity, participants perceived exercise during the exergaming modes to be most pleasant (P < 0.01), with greater enjoyment (P < 0.01) and dissociation (P < 0.01) in the game than track mode, and both modes higher on these variables than the control mode. Findings support the use of exergaming as a strategy to encourage individuals to exercise, with participants choosing to work harder physiologically, but reporting more positive psychological responses during and following the exercise.

[1]  David M Williams,et al.  Does Affective Valence During and Immediately Following a 10-Min Walk Predict Concurrent and Future Physical Activity? , 2012, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[2]  P. Ekkekakis Pleasure and displeasure from the body: Perspectives from exercise , 2003, Cognition & emotion.

[3]  Gershon Tenenbaum,et al.  Handbook of Sport Psychology: Tenenbaum/Handbook , 2007 .

[4]  A. Bauman,et al.  Physical activity and public health: updated recommendation for adults from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association. , 2007, Circulation.

[5]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Emotion, Pain, and Physical Illness , 1979 .

[6]  S. Leatherdale,et al.  Active Video Games and Health Indicators in Children and Youth: A Systematic Review , 2013, PloS one.

[7]  Deborah Kendzierski,et al.  Physical Activity Enjoyment Scale: Two Validation Studies , 1991 .

[8]  M. Nyska,et al.  Tear of peroneus longus in long distance runners due to enlarged peroneal tubercle , 2014, BMC Sports Science, Medicine and Rehabilitation.

[9]  V. Tammen Elite middle and long distance runners associative/dissociative coping , 1996 .

[10]  Patterning of physiological and affective responses in older active adults during a maximal graded exercise test and self-selected exercise , 2015, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[11]  C. Hardy,et al.  Not What, but How One Feels: The Measurement of Affect during Exercise , 1989 .

[12]  R. Eston,et al.  Submaximal, Perceptually Regulated Exercise Testing Predicts Maximal Oxygen Uptake: A Meta-Analysis Study , 2016, Sports Medicine.

[13]  Deniz S. Ones,et al.  The effect of acute aerobic exercise on positive activated affect: A meta-analysis , 2006 .

[14]  Gershon Tenenbaum,et al.  Attention focus during physical effort: The mediating role of task intensity , 2007 .

[15]  William Bertucci,et al.  Interactive-Virtual Reality (IVR) Exercise: An Examination of In-Task and Pre-to-Post Exercise Affective Changes , 2011 .

[16]  Mark Newton,et al.  Short-Term Psychological Effects of Interactive Video Game Technology Exercise on Mood and Attention , 2008, J. Educ. Technol. Soc..

[17]  P. Ekkekakis,et al.  Can High-Intensity Exercise Be More Pleasant? Attentional Dissociation Using Music and Video. , 2014, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[18]  Alexander Woll,et al.  Reliability, validity, and measurement invariance of the German version of the physical activity enjoyment scale. , 2013, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[19]  R. Eston,et al.  Use of a perceptually-regulated test to measure maximal oxygen uptake is valid and feels better , 2014, European journal of sport science.

[20]  David M Williams,et al.  Self-efficacy versus perceived enjoyment as predictors of physical activity behaviour , 2016, Psychology & health.

[21]  S. Petruzzello,et al.  More isn’t necessarily better: Examining the intensity–affect–enjoyment relationship in the context of resistance exercise. , 2015 .

[22]  C. Karageorghis,et al.  See Hear: Psychological Effects of Music and Music-Video During Treadmill Running , 2015, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[23]  Carrie Ferguson,et al.  Affective responses of inactive women to a maximal incremental exercise test: A test of the dual-mode model , 2007 .

[24]  I. Ferrera,et al.  Transient Changes in Bacterioplankton Communities Induced by the Submarine Volcanic Eruption of El Hierro (Canary Islands) , 2015, PloS one.

[25]  P. Ekkekakis Physiological and Psychological Evidence for the Potential of Self-Selected Exercise Intensity in Public Health , 2009 .

[26]  E. Deci,et al.  The "What" and "Why" of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior , 2000 .

[27]  P. Ekkekakis,et al.  Do ‘Mind over Muscle’ Strategies Work? , 2009, Sports medicine.

[28]  L. Dalleck,et al.  Is a threshold-based model a superior method to the relative percent concept for establishing individual exercise intensity? a randomized controlled trial , 2015, BMC Sports Science, Medicine and Rehabilitation.

[29]  R. Eston,et al.  A perceptually regulated, graded exercise test predicts peak oxygen uptake during treadmill exercise in active and sedentary participants , 2012, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[30]  Javier Monedero,et al.  Interactive Video Game Cycling Leads to Higher Energy Expenditure and Is More Enjoyable than Conventional Exercise in Adults , 2015, PloS one.

[31]  R. Eston,et al.  Prediction of Maximal or Peak Oxygen Uptake from Ratings of Perceived Exertion , 2014, Sports Medicine.

[32]  P. Thompson,et al.  ACSM's Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription , 1995 .

[33]  R. Eklund,et al.  Handbook of sport psychology , 2007 .

[34]  G. Parfitt,et al.  Patterning of affective responses during a graded exercise test in children and adolescents. , 2012, Pediatric exercise science.

[35]  P. Ekkekakis,et al.  The Pleasure and Displeasure People Feel When they Exercise at Different Intensities , 2011, Sports medicine.

[36]  P. Ekkekakis,et al.  Throwing the mountains into the lakes: on the perils of nomothetic conceptions of the exercise-affect relationship. , 2000 .

[37]  C. Fairman,et al.  Affective Responses to Acute Resistance Exercise Performed at Self-Selected and Imposed Loads in Trained Women , 2015, Journal of strength and conditioning research.

[38]  Elaine A. Rose,et al.  A quantitative analysis and qualitative explanation of the individual differences in affective responses to prescribed and self-selected exercise intensities. , 2007, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[39]  Gaynor Parfitt,et al.  Acute affective responses to prescribed and self-selected exercise sessions in adolescent girls: an observational study , 2014, BMC Sports Science, Medicine and Rehabilitation.

[40]  T. Levine,et al.  Eta Squared, Partial Eta Squared, and Misreporting of Effect Size in Communication Research , 2002 .

[41]  Carminda Maria Goersch Fontenele Lamboglia,et al.  Exergaming as a Strategic Tool in the Fight against Childhood Obesity: A Systematic Review , 2013, Journal of obesity.

[42]  S. Gaskill,et al.  Validity and reliability of combining three methods to determine ventilatory threshold. , 2001, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[43]  Ryan E Rhodes,et al.  The health benefits of interactive video game exercise. , 2007, Applied physiology, nutrition, and metabolism = Physiologie appliquee, nutrition et metabolisme.

[44]  David M Williams,et al.  Acute Affective Response to a Moderate-intensity Exercise Stimulus Predicts Physical Activity Participation 6 and 12 Months Later. , 2008, Psychology of sport and exercise.

[45]  P. Ekkekakis,et al.  The affective beneficence of vigorous exercise revisited. , 2002, British journal of health psychology.

[46]  F. Nakamura,et al.  Self-selected or imposed exercise? A different approach for affective comparisons , 2015, Journal of sports sciences.

[47]  Costas I. Karageorghis,et al.  Music in the exercise domain: a review and synthesis (Part II) , 2011, International review of sport and exercise psychology.

[48]  Shigeru Inoue,et al.  The pandemic of physical inactivity: global action for public health , 2012, The Lancet.

[49]  David M Williams,et al.  Disentangling the effects of choice and intensity on affective response to and preference for self-selected- versus imposed-intensity physical activity , 2013 .

[50]  E. Heath Borg's Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales , 1998 .

[51]  Elaine A. Rose,et al.  The psychological and physiological responses of sedentary individuals to prescribed and preferred intensity exercise. , 2006, British journal of health psychology.

[52]  Gershon Tenenbaum,et al.  Exertion–Attention–Flow Linkage Under Different Workloads , 2010 .

[53]  G. Parfitt,et al.  Acute affective responses to prescribed and self-selected exercise intensities in young adolescent boys and girls. , 2008, Pediatric exercise science.