Affective habituation: subliminal exposure to extreme stimuli decreases their extremity.

Following a functional perspective on evaluation, the authors hypothesized that subliminal exposure to extreme stimuli (e.g., extremely negative or positive words) would lead these stimuli to be perceived as less extreme. This process--affective habituation--was tested in 4 experiments. In Experiment 1, participants were subliminally exposed to extremely positive and extremely negative words. In a subsequent explicit-judgment task, these words were rated as less extreme than extreme words that had not been presented. In Experiment 2, these results were replicated with an implicit evaluation measure. In Experiments 3 and 4, subliminal exposure to extreme positive and negative words made the words "behave" as words that are only moderately positive or negative. Several implications are discussed.

[1]  J. Pligt,et al.  The Referents of Trait Inferences: The Impact of Trait Concepts Versus Actor-Trait Links on Subsequent Judgments , 1996 .

[2]  S. Rauch,et al.  Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli , 2001, Neuroreport.

[3]  R. Bornstein,et al.  The Generalizability of Subliminal Mere Exposure Effects: Influence of Stimuli Perceived Without Awareness on Social Behavior , 1987 .

[4]  A. Ohman,et al.  "Unconscious anxiety": phobic responses to masked stimuli. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[5]  J. Stainer,et al.  The Emotions , 1922, Nature.

[6]  J. Wolpe,et al.  Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition , 1958, Conditional reflex.

[7]  H. Aarts,et al.  On Wildebeests and Humans , 2003, Psychological science.

[8]  Peter Shizgal,et al.  On the neural computation of utility: implications from studies of brain stimulation reward , 1999 .

[9]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Tom Postmes,et al.  Seeing One Thing and Doing Another : Contrast Effects in Automatic Behavior , 2004 .

[11]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  If Attitudes Affect How Stimuli Are Processed, Should They Not Affect the Event-Related Brain Potential? , 1993 .

[12]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. , 2001, Emotion.

[13]  David M. Sanbonmatsu,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  R. Davidson Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[15]  O. John,et al.  Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  R. Zajonc Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .

[17]  R. Fazio,et al.  Variability in the likelihood of automatic attitude activation: data reanalysis and commentary on Bargh, Chaiken, Govender, and Pratto (1992) , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  M. Cabanac Physiological Role of Pleasure , 1971, Science.

[19]  Sean A. Spence,et al.  Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain , 1995 .

[20]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. , 1999 .

[21]  R. Dantzer The Psychology of Fear and Stress, J.A. Gray (Ed.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1987), viii and 422 pp, ISBN 0-521-27098-7 , 1989 .

[22]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Habituation of fear: effects of intensity and stimulus order. , 1974, Psychophysiology.

[24]  R. Zajonc Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferences , 1980 .

[25]  Mary Coverjo Nes,et al.  A Laboratory Study of Fear: The Case Of Peter , 1924 .

[26]  R. Fazio On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview , 2001 .

[27]  M. Banaji,et al.  When fair is foul and foul is fair: reverse priming in automatic evaluation. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion, novelty, and the startle reflex: habituation in humans. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[29]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Subliminal Mere Exposure: Specific, General, and Diffuse Effects , 2000, Psychological science.

[30]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Affective Discrimination of Stimuli That Cannot Be Recognized , 2022 .

[31]  J. Eccles The emotional brain. , 1980, Bulletin et memoires de l'Academie royale de medecine de Belgique.

[32]  A. Cooper,et al.  Consequences of Priming : Judgment and Behavior , 2004 .

[33]  Julia L. Shields Fair Is Foul. , 1981 .

[34]  D. Hermans,et al.  The affective priming effect: Automatic activation of evaluative information in memory. , 1994 .

[35]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The Automatic Evaluation Effect: Unconditional Automatic Attitude Activation with a Pronunciation Task , 1996 .

[36]  B. Campbell,et al.  Autonomic and behavioral correlates of appetitive conditioning in rats. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[37]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant stimuli. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[39]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorizations: I. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.