The development of cognitive empathy and concern in preschool children: A behavioral neuroscience investigation.

This developmental neuroscience study examined the electrophysiological responses (EEG and ERPs) associated with perspective taking and empathic concern in preschool children, as well as their relation to parental empathy dispositions and children's own prosocial behavior. Consistent with a body of previous studies using stimuli depicting somatic pain in both children and adults, larger early (~200 ms) ERPs were identified when perceiving painful versus neutral stimuli. In the slow wave window (~800 ms), a significant interaction of empathy condition and stimulus type was driven by a greater difference between painful and neutral images in the empathic concern condition. Across early development, children exhibited enhanced N2 to pain when engaging in empathic concern. Greater pain-elicited N2 responses in the cognitive empathy condition also related to parent dispositional empathy. Children's own prosocial behavior was predicted by several individual differences in neural function, including larger early LPP responses during cognitive empathy and greater differentiation in late LPP and slow wave responses to empathic concern versus affective perspective taking. Left frontal activation (greater alpha suppression) while engaging in affective perspective taking was also related to higher levels of parent cognitive empathy. Together, this multilevel analysis demonstrates the important distinction between facets of empathy in children; the value of examining neurobehavioral processes in development. It provides provoking links between children's neural functioning and parental dispositions in early development.

[1]  B. Miller,et al.  Deconstructing empathy: Neuroanatomical dissociations between affect sharing and prosocial motivation using a patient lesion model , 2017, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  Ted Ruffman,et al.  Theory of mind and prosocial behavior in childhood: A meta-analytic review. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[3]  R. Salekin Psychopathy in childhood: Toward better informing the DSM-5 and ICD-11 conduct disorder specifiers. , 2016, Personality disorders.

[4]  J. Decety,et al.  Precursors to morality in development as a complex interplay between neural, socioenvironmental, and behavioral facets , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[5]  J. Decety,et al.  Specific electrophysiological components disentangle affective sharing and empathic concern in psychopathy. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  J. Decety The neural pathways, development and functions of empathy , 2015, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[7]  Trent S. Parker,et al.  Adolescents’ and Parents’ Expression of Empathy: A Social Neuroscience Perspective , 2015 .

[8]  J. Decety,et al.  An EEG/ERP investigation of the development of empathy in early and middle childhood , 2014, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  J. Decety,et al.  Friends or Foes , 2014, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[10]  Jean Decety,et al.  Spatiotemporal neural dynamics of moral judgment: A high-density ERP study , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  J. Decety,et al.  The complex relation between morality and empathy , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Sunil Kumar Meena,et al.  Electricity from the Silk Cocoon Membrane , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[13]  C. Moore,et al.  The influence of empathic concern on prosocial behavior in children , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[14]  J. Decety,et al.  Aging is associated with changes in the neural circuits underlying empathy , 2014, Neurobiology of Aging.

[15]  J. Decety,et al.  Oxytocin receptor gene variation predicts empathic concern and autonomic arousal while perceiving harm to others , 2014, Social neuroscience.

[16]  J. Decety,et al.  Empathic responsiveness in amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex in youths with psychopathic traits. , 2013, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[17]  J. Decety,et al.  Experiencing a Natural Disaster Alters Children’s Altruistic Giving , 2013, Psychological science.

[18]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Concern for Others in the First Year of Life: Theory, Evidence, and Avenues for Research , 2013 .

[19]  T. Malti,et al.  The relation of moral emotion attributions to prosocial and antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis. , 2013, Child development.

[20]  Daniel Grühn,et al.  Empathic concern and perspective taking: linear and quadratic effects of age across the adult life span. , 2013, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[21]  Jean Decety,et al.  The speed of morality: a high-density electrical neuroimaging study. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  A. Ibanez,et al.  What event-related potentials (ERPs) bring to social neuroscience? , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[23]  A. Samek,et al.  Exploring the Origins of Charitable Acts: Evidence from an Artefactual Field Experiment with Young Children , 2012 .

[24]  Michael Inzlicht,et al.  Right frontal cortical asymmetry predicts empathic reactions: support for a link between withdrawal motivation and empathy. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[25]  Jean Decety,et al.  Voice and Emotion Processing in the Human Neonatal Brain , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Decety,et al.  Dissociation between affective sharing and emotion understanding in juvenile psychopaths , 2012, Development and Psychopathology.

[27]  Murray T. Maybery,et al.  Empathy, Perspective Taking and Prosocial Behaviour: The Importance of Parenting Practices , 2012 .

[28]  Rishi Bhardwaj,et al.  Infant Pupil Diameter Changes in Response to Others' Positive and Negative Emotions , 2011, PloS one.

[29]  Peter Goldie,et al.  EmpathyPhilosophical and Psychological Perspectives , 2011 .

[30]  C. Moore,et al.  The behavioural expression of empathy to others’ pain versus others’ sadness in young children , 2011, PAIN.

[31]  Claus Lamm,et al.  Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain , 2011, NeuroImage.

[32]  Angela D. Evans,et al.  The neural correlates of reasoning about prosocial–helping decisions: An event-related brain potentials study , 2011, Brain Research.

[33]  Jean Decety,et al.  Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood. , 2010, Developmental science.

[34]  G. Kochanska,et al.  Children's conscience during toddler and preschool years, moral self, and a competent, adaptive developmental trajectory. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[35]  J. Decety The Neurodevelopment of Empathy in Humans , 2010, Developmental Neuroscience.

[36]  Jean Decety,et al.  Physicians down-regulate their pain empathy response: An event-related brain potential study , 2010, NeuroImage.

[37]  Wei Li,et al.  Perspective taking modulates event-related potentials to perceived pain , 2010, Neuroscience Letters.

[38]  G. Hajcak,et al.  The late positive potential: a neurophysiological marker for emotion regulation in children. , 2009, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[39]  Richard J Davidson,et al.  Empathy is associated with dynamic change in prefrontal brain electrical activity during positive emotion in children. , 2009, Child development.

[40]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Empathy in Early Childhood , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[41]  Dan Foti,et al.  Motivated and controlled attention to emotion: Time-course of the late positive potential , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[42]  Greg Hajcak,et al.  Brain potentials during affective picture processing in children , 2009, Biological Psychology.

[43]  M. Carpenter,et al.  Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[44]  R. Corcoran,et al.  The QCAE: A Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy , 2009, European Psychiatry.

[45]  P. Zelazo,et al.  The time course of social-emotional processing in early childhood: ERP responses to facial affect and personal familiarity in a Go-Nogo task , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: Genetic and environmental contributions. , 2008, Emotion.

[47]  Jean Decety,et al.  Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Jean Decety,et al.  From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.

[49]  Aaron D. J. Frost,et al.  A Measure of Cognitive and Affective Empathy in Children Using Parent Ratings , 2008, Child psychiatry and human development.

[50]  U. Fischbacher,et al.  Oxytocin Shapes the Neural Circuitry of Trust and Trust Adaptation in Humans , 2008, Neuron.

[51]  Po-Lei Lee,et al.  The perception of pain in others suppresses somatosensory oscillations: A magnetoencephalography study , 2008, NeuroImage.

[52]  P. Frick,et al.  Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. , 2008, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[53]  Jonas K. Olofsson,et al.  Affective picture processing: An integrative review of ERP findings , 2008, Biological Psychology.

[54]  Lihua Mao,et al.  Gender difference in empathy for pain: An electrophysiological investigation , 2008, Brain Research.

[55]  R. Simons,et al.  Modulations of the electrophysiological response to pleasant stimuli by cognitive reappraisal. , 2008, Emotion.

[56]  J. Benenson,et al.  Children's altruistic behavior in the dictator game , 2007 .

[57]  R. E. Wheeler,et al.  Frontal brain asymmetry and emotional reactivity: a biological substrate of affective style. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[58]  Stefanie Hoehl,et al.  Neural mechanisms of joint attention in infancy , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[59]  Joshua Knobe,et al.  Acting Intentionally and the Side-Effect Effect , 2006, Psychological science.

[60]  L. Konopka,et al.  Frontal Alpha Power Asymmetry in Aggressive Children and Adolescents with Mood and Disruptive Behavior Disorders , 2006, Clinical EEG and neuroscience.

[61]  H. Demaree,et al.  Brain lateralization of emotional processing: historical roots and a future incorporating "dominance". , 2005, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[62]  P. McGuffin,et al.  The development of prosocial behaviour in children and adolescents: a twin study. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[63]  J. Decety,et al.  The functional architecture of human empathy. , 2004, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[64]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[65]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  Consistency and development of prosocial dispositions: a longitudinal study. , 1999, Child development.

[66]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Asymmetric brain function, affective style, and psychopathology: The role of early experience and plasticity , 1994, Development and Psychopathology.

[67]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  The development of empathy in twins. , 1992 .

[68]  R. Davidson Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[69]  Richard A. Fabes,et al.  Empathy: Conceptualization, measurement, and relation to prosocial behavior , 1990 .

[70]  P. Ekman,et al.  Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: emotional expression and brain physiology. I. , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[71]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[72]  S. Scarr,et al.  How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype greater than environment effects. , 1983, Child development.

[73]  D. Hay,et al.  Responses of six-month-olds to the distress of their peers. , 1981, Child development.

[74]  P. Visscher,et al.  Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. , 2015, Nature genetics.

[75]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Age-related sex differences in explicit measures of empathy do not predict brain responses across childhood and adolescence , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Neural development of mentalizing in moral judgment from adolescence to adulthood , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[77]  J. Decety,et al.  Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[78]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  The contribution of emotion and cognition to moral sensitivity: a neurodevelopmental study. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[79]  Nancy Eisenberg,et al.  Empathic responding: Sympathy and personal distress. , 2009 .

[80]  J. Decety,et al.  Empathy versus personal distress: Recent evidence from social neuroscience. , 2009 .

[81]  C. Batson These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. , 2009 .

[82]  T. Moffitt,et al.  Heritability of antisocial behaviour at 9: do callous-unemotional traits matter? , 2008, Developmental science.

[83]  Robert Plomin,et al.  Prosocial behavior from early to middle childhood: genetic and environmental influences on stability and change. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[84]  J. Pickens,et al.  EEG Stability in Infants/Children of Depressed Mothers , 1997, Child psychiatry and human development.

[85]  N. Eisenberg,et al.  A multiplicative model of the dispositional antecedents of a prosocial behavior: predicting more of the people more of the time. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[86]  C. Zahn-Waxler,et al.  Development of concern for others. , 1992 .

[87]  Tracy A. Dennis,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Neural Correlates of Cognitive Reappraisal in Children: an Erp Study , 2022 .