How wild bearded capuchin monkeys select stones and nuts to minimize the number of strikes per nut cracked

Wild bearded capuchin monkeys, Cebus libidinosus, use stone tools to crack palm nuts to obtain the kernel. In five experiments, we gave 10 monkeys from one wild group of bearded capuchins a choice of two nuts differing in resistance and size and/or two manufactured stones of the same shape, volume and composition but different mass. Monkeys consistently selected the nut that was easier to crack and the heavier stone. When choosing between two stones differing in mass by a ratio of 1.3:1, monkeys frequently touched the stones or tapped them with their fingers or with a nut. They showed these behaviours more frequently before making their first selection of a stone than afterward. These results suggest that capuchins discriminate between nuts and between stones, selecting materials that allow them to crack nuts with fewer strikes, and generate exploratory behaviours to discriminate stones of varying mass. In the final experiment, humans effectively discriminated the mass of stones using the same tapping and handling behaviours as capuchins. Capuchins explore objects in ways that allow them to perceive invariant properties (e.g. mass) of objects, enabling selection of objects for specific uses. We predict that species that use tools will generate behaviours that reveal invariant properties of objects such as mass; species that do not use tools are less likely to explore objects in this way. The precision with which individuals can judge invariant properties may differ considerably, and this also should predict prevalence of tool use across species.

[1]  F. A. King,et al.  Current perspectives in primate social dynamics , 1986 .

[2]  E. Wasserman,et al.  Comparative cognition : experimental explorations of animal intelligence , 2009 .

[3]  Valentina Truppa,et al.  Selection of Effective Stone Tools by Wild Bearded Capuchin Monkeys , 2009, Current Biology.

[4]  H. Jane Brockmann,et al.  Kleptoparasitism in birds , 1979, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  C. Boesch,et al.  Optimisation of Nut-Cracking With Natural Hammers By Wild Chimpanzees , 1983 .

[6]  D. Fragaszy,et al.  Acquisition of foraging competence in wild brown capuchins ( Cebus apella ), with special reference to conspecifics' foraging artefacts as an indirect social influence , 2008 .

[7]  M. Turvey Dynamic touch. , 1996, The American psychologist.

[8]  C. Peters,et al.  Nut-like oil seeds: food for monkeys, chimpanzees, humans, and probably ape-men. , 1987, American journal of physical anthropology.

[9]  J. Call,et al.  Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) , 2009, Biology Letters.

[10]  Christie Allan,et al.  The Cerrados of Brazil: Ecology and Natural History of a Neotropical Savanna , 2003 .

[11]  D. Fragaszy,et al.  Relational spatial reasoning by a nonhuman: the example of capuchin monkeys. , 2005, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[12]  P. Lucas,et al.  Seed-breaking forces exerted by orang-utans with their teeth in captivity and a new technique for estimating forces produced in the wild. , 1994, American journal of physical anthropology.

[13]  Dorothy Fragaszy,et al.  Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools , 2004, American journal of primatology.

[14]  Evolutionarily stable kleptoparasitism: consequences of different prey types , 2003 .

[15]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Primate Cognition , 2010, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[16]  C. Sanz,et al.  Chimpanzees prey on army ants with specialized tool set , 2010, American journal of primatology.

[17]  Jackie Chappell,et al.  Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides) , 2002, Animal Cognition.

[18]  Dorothy M. Fragaszy,et al.  What Is Challenging About Tool Use? The Capuchin’s Perspective , 2012 .

[19]  S. Malaivijitnond,et al.  The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long‐tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand , 2009, American journal of primatology.

[20]  Chris Carbone,et al.  Feeding success in African wild dogs : does kleptoparasitism by spotted hyenas influence hunting group size ? , 1997 .

[21]  M. E. Haas,et al.  Tap-Scanning for Invertebrates by Capuchins (Cebus apella) , 2003, Folia Primatologica.

[22]  B. Bertram,et al.  Egyptian Vultures Neophron percnopterus and Ostrich Struthio camelus eggs: the origins of stone-throwing behaviour , 2008 .

[23]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Bearded capuchin monkeys' and a human's efficiency at cracking palm nuts with stone tools: field experiments , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Kinematics and energetics of nut-cracking in wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in Piauí, Brazil. , 2009, American journal of physical anthropology.

[25]  E. P. Animal Behaviour , 1901, Nature.

[26]  D. Fragaszy,et al.  Manual exploratory procedures and asymmetries for a haptic search task: a comparison between capuchins (Cebus apella) and humans. , 1997, Laterality.

[27]  R. Klatzky,et al.  The Intelligent Hand , 1988 .

[28]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Characteristics of hammer stones and anvils used by wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) to crack open palm nuts. , 2007, American journal of physical anthropology.

[29]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  The Complete Capuchin: The Biology of the Genus Cebus , 2004 .

[30]  D. Fragaszy,et al.  Generative aspects of manipulation in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 1991, Journal of comparative psychology.

[31]  Jackie Chappell,et al.  Selection of tool diameter by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[32]  J. Gibson The concept of the stimulus in psychology. , 1960 .

[33]  Crickette M. Sanz,et al.  Flexible and Persistent Tool-using Strategies in Honey-gathering by Wild Chimpanzees , 2009, International Journal of Primatology.

[34]  B. O'Shaughnessy The sense of touch , 1989 .

[35]  E. Ottoni,et al.  Semifree-ranging Tufted Capuchins (Cebus apella) Spontaneously Use Tools to Crack Open Nuts , 2001, International Journal of Primatology.

[36]  J. Phillips-Conroy Foraging for Survival: Yearling Baboons in Africa. , 2000 .

[37]  M. Turvey,et al.  Weight perception and the haptic size-weight illusion are functions of the inertia tensor. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  T. Aumann,et al.  Use of Stones by the Black-Breasted Buzzard Hamirostra melanosternon to Gain Access to Egg Contents for Food , 1990 .

[39]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Physical properties of palm fruits processed with tools by wild bearded capuchins (Cebus libidinosus) , 2008, American journal of primatology.

[40]  Russell D. Gray,et al.  The right tool for the job: what strategies do wild New Caledonian crows use? , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[41]  S. Tebbich,et al.  Cognitive abilities related to tool use in the woodpecker finch, Cactospiza pallida , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  Gregory Burton,et al.  Non-Neural Extensions of haptic Sensitivity , 1993 .

[43]  D. Fragaszy,et al.  Generative aspects of manipulation in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 1991 .