Numerically estimating internal models of dynamic virtual objects

Precise manipulation of objects is ordinarily limited by visual, kinesthetic, motor, and cognitive factors. Specially designed virtual objects and tasks minimize such limitations, making it possible to isolate and estimate the internal model that guides subjects' performance. Subjects manipulated a computer-generated virtual object (vO), attempting to align vO to a target whose position changed randomly every 10 s. To analyze the control actions subjects use while manipulating the vO, we benchmarked human performance against that of ideal performers (IPs), behavioral counterparts to ideal observers used in sensory research. These comparisons showed that subjects performed as feed-forward, predictive controllers. Simulations with degraded-IPs suggest that human asymptotic performance was not limited by imprecisions of vision or of motor timing, but resulted mainly from inaccuracies in the internal models of vO dynamics.

[1]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  S. Keele,et al.  Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: a correctional analysis. , 1985, Acta psychologica.

[4]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[5]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Learning arm kinematics and dynamics. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[8]  W. Geisler Sequential ideal-observer analysis of visual discriminations. , 1989, Psychological review.

[9]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  F. A. Seiler,et al.  Numerical Recipes in C: The Art of Scientific Computing , 1989 .

[11]  Acknowledgments , 2006, Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology.

[12]  R C Miall,et al.  The cerebellum, predictive control and motor coordination. , 2007, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[13]  G. Bock,et al.  Sensory guidance of movement , 1998 .

[14]  R C Miall,et al.  System Identification Applied to a Visuomotor Task: Near-Optimal Human Performance in a Noisy Changing Task , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  P. R. Davidson,et al.  Motor learning and prediction in a variable environment , 2003, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  Visual detection of motion , 1994 .

[17]  Hiroshi Imamizu,et al.  Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool , 2000, Nature.

[18]  R. Altes An interpretation of cortical maps in echolocating bats. , 1989, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[19]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Motor prediction , 2001, Current Biology.

[20]  William H. Press,et al.  Numerical recipes , 1990 .

[21]  M Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control. , 2007, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[22]  S. Mateeff,et al.  The time it takes to detect changes in speed and direction of visual motion , 1998, Vision Research.

[23]  D M Wolpert,et al.  The influence of previous experience on predictive motor control , 2001, Neuroreport.

[24]  A. G. Witney,et al.  Learning and decay of prediction in object manipulation. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  D M Levi,et al.  Spatial alignment across gaps: contributions of orientation and spatial scale. , 1995, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[26]  R. Johansson,et al.  Prediction Precedes Control in Motor Learning , 2003, Current Biology.

[27]  C. A. Burbeck,et al.  Exposure-duration effects in localization judgments. , 1986, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[28]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Detection of changes in speed and direction of motion: Reaction time analysis , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Psychophysics of Motion Perception , 1982 .

[30]  D. Lieberman,et al.  Fourier analysis , 2004, Journal of cataract and refractive surgery.

[31]  C. A. Burbeck,et al.  Two mechanisms for localization? Evidence for separation-dependent and separation-independent processing of position information , 1990, Vision Research.

[32]  W. Press,et al.  Numerical Recipes in C++: The Art of Scientific Computing (2nd edn)1 Numerical Recipes Example Book (C++) (2nd edn)2 Numerical Recipes Multi-Language Code CD ROM with LINUX or UNIX Single-Screen License Revised Version3 , 2003 .

[33]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  H. Barlow The absolute efficiency of perceptual decisions. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[35]  C. A. Burbeck Position and spatial frequency in large-scale localization judgments , 1987, Vision Research.