Phonological processing, grammar and sentence comprehension in older and younger generations of Swedish children with cochlear implants

Background and aims Phonological processing skills measured by nonword repetition, are consistently found to be hampered in children with severe/profound hearing impairment and cochlear implants, compared to children with normal hearing. Many studies also find that grammar is affected. There are no studies exploring grammar in the Swedish population of children with cochlear implants. Documentation is also sparse regarding if and how language development in children with cochlear implants at the group level has changed over time with for example earlier implantation. The aim of the present study is to explore nonword repetition, grammatical production and sentence comprehension in an older generation of Swedish children with cochlear implants implanted during the 1990s and in a younger generation implanted after 2004. We also wanted to find out if and how nonword repetition is associated with grammatical production and comprehension in the two generations, taking the role of speech perception into consideration. Methods Thirteen adolescents with severe/profound hearing impairment and unilateral cochlear implants, aged 11;9 to 19;1 at the time of testing (age at implant 2;5 to 11;11) and 16 children with severe/profound hearing impairment and cochlear implants, aged 5;3 to 8;0 (age at implant 0;7 to 5;6, ten bilateral) participated. All participants used oral communication. They were tested with nonword repetition and sentence comprehension tasks. Language samples for grammatical analysis were collected during a referential communication task. Transcriptions were analysed with respect to mean length of utterance and grammatical accuracy. Results The two groups performed similar to each other and to reference data from much younger children with normal hearing and language development on nonword repetition. Both groups showed problems in grammatical accuracy. The majority of grammatical errors involved grammatical morphemes. All participants in the older group had significant problems with sentence comprehension, whereas variation was large in the younger group, some children performing at age level. In both groups, nonword repetition was associated with grammatical accuracy and in the younger group also with sentence comprehension. Conclusions Phonological processing skills are significantly hampered in children with cochlear implants, with consequences for language processing and development. Their grammatical problems involve the use of grammatical morphemes, similar to what is found for hearing children with specific language impairment. In spite of early implantation, the results from the younger group indicate that this is still a group at risk for problems with language learning. Implications Careful follow-up and support of language development in children with cochlear implants is crucial to identify children, whose problems are persistent. It is important for speech-language pathologists to take the interdependency of speech perception, phonological processing skills and other language skills into account.

[1]  Bente Eriksen Hagtvet,et al.  Listening comprehension and reading comprehension in poor decoders: Evidence for the importance of syntactic and semantic skills as well as phonological skills , 2003 .

[2]  A. Wingfield,et al.  The Two Sides of Sensory–Cognitive Interactions: Effects of Age, Hearing Acuity, and Working Memory Span on Sentence Comprehension , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[4]  U Nettelbladt,et al.  Non-word repetition in children with language impairment--pitfalls and possibilities. , 1999, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[5]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Nonword repetition by children with cochlear implants: accuracy ratings from normal-hearing listeners. , 2004, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[6]  Joanna H. Lowenstein,et al.  Nonword repetition in children with cochlear implants: a potential clinical marker of poor language acquisition. , 2014, American journal of speech-language pathology.

[7]  David B. Pisoni,et al.  Language Development in Profoundly Deaf Children with Cochlear Implants , 2000, Psychological science.

[8]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The phonological loop as a language learning device. , 1998, Psychological review.

[9]  B. MacWhinney The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk , 1992 .

[10]  J. Tomblin,et al.  Acquisition of tense marking in English-speaking children with cochlear implants: a longitudinal study. , 2013, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.

[11]  G. Conti-Ramsden,et al.  Non-word repetition and grammatical morphology: normative data for children in their final year of primary school. , 2001, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[12]  Christopher M. Conway,et al.  Learning and Memory Processes Following Cochlear Implantation: The Missing Piece of the Puzzle , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[13]  M. Svirsky,et al.  Grammatical Morphologic Development in Pediatric Cochlear Implant Users May Be Affected by the Perceptual Prominence of the Relevant Markers , 2002, The Annals of otology, rhinology & laryngology. Supplement.

[14]  A. Geers,et al.  Spoken language scores of children using cochlear implants compared to hearing age-mates at school entry. , 2009, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.

[15]  L B Leonard,et al.  Specific language impairment in Swedish: the status of verb morphology and word order. , 2000, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[16]  Laurence B Leonard,et al.  Specific Language Impairment Across Languages. , 2014, Child development perspectives.

[17]  P. Spencer,et al.  Individual differences in language performance after cochlear implantation at one to three years of age: child, family, and linguistic factors. , 2004, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.

[18]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Imitation of nonwords by hearing impaired children with cochlear implants: suprasegmental analyses , 2002, Clinical linguistics & phonetics.

[19]  B. McMurray,et al.  Longitudinal Speech Perception and Language Performance in Pediatric Cochlear Implant Users: The Effect of Age at Implantation , 2014, Ear and hearing.

[20]  Anu Sharma,et al.  Cross-Modal Re-Organization in Clinical Populations with Hearing Loss , 2016, Brain sciences.

[21]  J. Locke A Theory of Neurolinguistic Development , 1997, Brain and Language.

[22]  Lisa Davidson,et al.  Persistent Language Delay Versus Late Language Emergence in Children With Early Cochlear Implantation. , 2016, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[23]  Gisela Szagun,et al.  Learning by ear: on the acquisition of case and gender marking by German-speaking children with normal hearing and with cochlear implants. , 2004, Journal of child language.

[24]  P. Wong,et al.  Training to Improve Language Outcomes in Cochlear Implant Recipients , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[25]  Elina Mäki-Torkko,et al.  Cognitive and linguistic skills in Swedish children with cochlear implants - measures of accuracy and latency as indicators of development. , 2008, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[26]  B. Sahlén,et al.  Can severe language disorders be identified before age 3;6? , 1996 .

[27]  P. Govaerts,et al.  The development of oral language in children with bilateral hearing loss: From speech perception to morphosyntax. , 2014 .

[28]  N. Fox,et al.  A Comparison of the Speech and Language Skills of Children With Cochlear Implants and Children With Normal Hearing , 2008 .

[29]  Caitlin A. Rice,et al.  Language Structures Used by Kindergartners With Cochlear Implants: Relationship to Phonological Awareness, Lexical Knowledge and Hearing Loss , 2014, Ear and hearing.

[30]  David B Pisoni,et al.  Neurocognitive factors in sensory restoration of early deafness: a connectome model , 2016, The Lancet Neurology.

[31]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Nonword imitation by children with cochlear implants: consonant analyses. , 2004, Archives of Otolaryngology - Head and Neck Surgery.

[32]  C Donlan,et al.  Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[33]  Julian M. Pine,et al.  Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. , 2004 .

[34]  Lynne E. Hewitt,et al.  Children with Specific Language Impairment , 2002 .

[35]  A methodological contribution to the assessment of nonword repetition—a comparison between children with specific language impairment and hearing-impaired children with hearing aids or cochlear implants , 2008, Logopedics, phoniatrics, vocology.

[36]  J. Wouters,et al.  Expressive vocabulary, morphology, syntax and narrative skills in profoundly deaf children after early cochlear implantation. , 2013, Research in developmental disabilities.

[37]  A. Löfqvist,et al.  Is age at implant the only factor that counts? The influence of working memory on lexical and grammatical development in children with cochlear implants , 2004, International journal of audiology.

[38]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. , 1992 .

[39]  Walter Loban,et al.  Language Development: Kindergarten through Grade Twelve. NCTE Committee on Research Report No. 18. , 1976 .

[40]  D. Killen,et al.  Receptive and Expressive Language Skills of Children with Five Years of Experience Using a Cochlear Implant , 2002, The Annals of otology, rhinology, and laryngology.

[41]  G. Conti-Ramsden,et al.  Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). , 2001, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[42]  E. Bates,et al.  Continuity in lexical and morphological development: a test of the critical mass hypothesis , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[43]  Ann E Geers,et al.  Will they catch up? The role of age at cochlear implantation in the spoken language development of children with severe to profound hearing loss. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[44]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? ☆ , 1990 .

[45]  Nataša Marić,et al.  Improving comprehension in adolescents with severe receptive language impairments: a randomized control trial of intervention for coordinating conjunctions. , 2014, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[46]  Robert Needlman,et al.  Speech and Language Impairments in Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention, and Outcome , 2001 .

[47]  G. Szagun Language Acquisition in Young German- Speaking Children with Cochlear Implants: Individual Differences and Implications for Conceptions of a ‘Sensitive Phase’ , 2001, Audiology and Neurotology.

[48]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[49]  S. Gathercole Complexities and constraints in nonword repetition and word learning , 2006, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[50]  Cecilia Nakeva von Mentzer,et al.  Hearing and cognitive development in deaf and hearing-impaired children: effects of intervention , 2013 .

[51]  Magnus Haake,et al.  The slower the better? Does the speaker's speech rate influence children's performance on a language comprehension test? , 2014, International journal of speech-language pathology.

[52]  David B Pisoni,et al.  Cochlear implants and spoken language processing abilities: review and assessment of the literature. , 2010, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[53]  Elizabeth D. Casserly,et al.  Nonword Repetition as a Predictor of Long-Term Speech and Language Skills in Children With Cochlear Implants , 2013, Otology & neurotology : official publication of the American Otological Society, American Neurotology Society [and] European Academy of Otology and Neurotology.

[54]  M. Ullman,et al.  Specific Language Impairment is not Specific to Language: the Procedural Deficit Hypothesis , 2005, Cortex.

[55]  S. Gathercole Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship , 2006, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[56]  S. Chiat The developmental trajectory of nonword repetition , 2006, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[57]  D. Pisoni,et al.  Speech timing and working memory in profoundly deaf children after cochlear implantation. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[58]  Kristina Hansson,et al.  Noun phrase morphology in Swedish-speaking children with specific language impairment , 2001, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[59]  D V Bishop,et al.  Language-impaired 4-year-olds: distinguishing transient from persistent impairment. , 1987, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[60]  M. Westerlund Identifying children at risk for language impairment: screening of communication at 18 months , 2004, Acta paediatrica.

[61]  Andrea D. Warner-Czyz,et al.  Cochlear implantation updates: the Dallas Cochlear Implant Program. , 2012, Journal of the American Academy of Audiology.

[62]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[63]  M. Svirsky,et al.  An Exploratory Look at Pediatric Cochlear Implantation: Is Earliest Always Best? , 2008, Ear and hearing.

[64]  Christopher M. Conway,et al.  Deaf children with cochlear implants do not appear to use sentence context to help recognize spoken words. , 2014, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[65]  S. Gathercole,et al.  Limitations in working memory: implications for language development. , 2000, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[66]  María Teresa Martín-Aragoneses,et al.  Morpho-syntactic reading comprehension in children with early and late cochlear implants. , 2015, Journal of deaf studies and deaf education.