Modelling stereokinetic phenomena by a minimum relative motion assumption: the tilted disk, the ellipsoid and the tilted bar
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Scott O. Murray,et al. Processing Shape, Motion and Three-dimensional Shape-from-motion in the Human Cortex , 2003 .
[2] S. Ullman,et al. The interpretation of visual motion , 1977 .
[3] Dejan Todorovic,et al. Analysis of two- and three-dimensional rigid and nonrigid motions in the stereokinetic effect , 1993 .
[4] J E Cutting,et al. Perception and information. , 1987, Annual review of psychology.
[5] Mario Zanforlin,et al. Quantitative determination of the three-dimensional appearances of a rotating ellipse without a rigidity assumption , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.
[6] H. F. J. M. Buffart,et al. Formal theories of visual perception , 1978 .
[7] M. Zanforlin. The height of a stereokinetic cone: A quantitative determination of a 3-D effect from 2-D moving patterns without a “rigidity assumption” , 1988, Psychological research.
[8] R. B. Mefferd. Perception of Depth in Rotating Objects: 7. Influence of Attributes of Depth on Stereokinetic Percepts , 1968 .
[9] J. Farrell,et al. Visual transformations underlying apparent movement , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[10] A. Cowey,et al. Blindness to form from motion despite intact static form perception and motion detection , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[11] R. Shepard. Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.
[12] James E Cutting,et al. An Invariant for Wheel-Generated Motions and the Logic of its Determination , 1980, Perception.
[13] R. B. Mefferd,et al. Perception of Depth in Rotating Objects: 4. Fluctuating Stereokinetic Perceptual Variants , 1968, Perceptual and motor skills.
[14] D. Gilden,et al. The effect of configural orientation on perceived trajectory in apparent motion , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[15] J. Cutting,et al. The minimum principle and the perception of absolute, common, and relative motions , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[16] G Johansson,et al. Vector analysis in visual perception of rolling motion , 1974, Psychologische Forschung.
[17] O. Reiser,et al. Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .
[18] Peter Ulric Tse,et al. The duration of 3-D form analysis in transformational apparent motion , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[19] G J Andersen,et al. A Counterexample to the Rigidity Assumption in the Visual Perception of Structure from Motion , 1984, Perception.
[20] S Ullman,et al. Rigidity and Misperceived Motion , 1984, Perception.
[21] R. Shepard. Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.
[22] G. Vallortigara,et al. Depth effect from a rotating line of constant length , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[23] D. Foster. Visual apparent motion and the calculus of variations , 1978 .
[24] Mario Zanforlin,et al. Analytic determination of the depth effect in stereokinetic phenomena without a rigidity assumption , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.
[25] S. Gunnar O. Johansson,et al. Configurations in event perception : an experimental study , 1951 .
[26] P Bressan,et al. Perceptual Alternations in Stereokinesis , 1988, Perception.
[27] Carsten Nicolas Boehler,et al. Binding 3-D Object Perception in the Human Visual Cortex , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] V. Bruce,et al. Visual Perception: Physiology, Psychology and Ecology , 1985 .
[29] K. Duncker,et al. Über induzierte Bewegung , 1929 .
[30] S Ullman,et al. Maximizing Rigidity: The Incremental Recovery of 3-D Structure from Rigid and Nonrigid Motion , 1984, Perception.
[31] P. Bressan,et al. Multiple 3-D Interpretations in a Classic Stereokinetic Effect , 1986, Perception.