Analyzing ‘Spooky Action at a Distance' Concerning Brand Logos

Attractive brand logos and packaging elements are gaining more and more importance as decisive competitive advantages in a world flooded by stimuli. Based on the assumption that there exists a kind of collective knowledge beyond individual experience, the authors found that in respect to logos humans are more likely to respond to stimuli if many people in other parts of the world do or did know them, even though they personally are not consciously familiar with the logos. An improved favorability of 20% for original symbols versus comparable control symbols can be regarded as a solid competitive advantage. This benefit regarding brand logos was analyzed by means of latent class models. Additionally, the heterogeneity in the participant’s characteristics as well as the heterogeneity in the analyzed symbols were incorporated by means of random and fixed effects models. Furthermore, this effect was shown to be neither culture-specific nor linked to age, gender, level of extraversion, and education of the participants.

[1]  Keith C. Williams,et al.  Behavioural Aspects of Marketing , 1981 .

[2]  B. Baltagi,et al.  Econometric Analysis of Panel Data , 2020, Springer Texts in Business and Economics.

[3]  Hans J. Eysenck,et al.  The Eysenck Personality Inventory , 1965 .

[4]  John R. Anderson Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications , 1980 .

[5]  Robert A. Bjork,et al.  Measures of Memory , 1988 .

[6]  N. Brody,et al.  Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized: effects of shadowing, masking, and cerebral laterality. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  Cheng Hsiao,et al.  Analysis of Panel Data , 1987 .

[8]  Kevin Lane Keller Branding and brand equity , 2002 .

[9]  R. Bornstein Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. , 1989 .

[10]  Brian T. Ratchford,et al.  The Economics of Consumer Knowledge , 2001 .

[11]  R. Kronauer,et al.  Affective Discrimination of Stimuli That Cannot Be Recognized , 2022 .

[12]  George Mandler,et al.  Nonspecific Effects of Exposure on Stimuli That Cannot Be Recognized , 1987 .

[13]  N. Brody,et al.  Critical importance of exposure duration for affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  Chris Janiszewski,et al.  The Influence of Print Advertisement Organization on Affect Toward a Brand Name , 1990 .

[15]  B. Schlink The presence of the past , 2009 .

[16]  T. Meyvis,et al.  Effects of Brand Logo Complexity, Repetition, and Spacing on Processing Fluency and Judgment , 2001 .

[17]  G. Lefrançois Psychological theories and human learning: Kongor's report , 1972 .

[18]  A. Kuehn Consumer Brand Choice--A Learning Process? , 1976 .

[19]  G. Bonanno,et al.  Preference, familiarity, and recognition after repeated brief exposures to random geometric shapes. , 1986, The American journal of psychology.

[20]  Arthur S. Reber,et al.  Cognition Unawares. (Book Reviews: Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge. An Essay on the Cognitive Unconscious.) , 1996 .

[21]  Z. Dienes,et al.  Implicit learning: Below the subjective threshold , 1997 .

[22]  R. Zajonc Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .

[23]  D. Aaker Building Strong Brands , 1995 .

[24]  C. P. Goodman,et al.  The Tacit Dimension , 2003 .

[25]  R. Moreland,et al.  Is stimulus recognition a necessary condition for the occurrence of exposure effects? , 1977, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  J. E. Mazur,et al.  Learning and Behavior , 1966 .

[27]  Thomas T. Lawson,et al.  Archetypes and the collective unconscious , 2018 .