Relative advantages of dichromatic and trichromatic color vision in camouflage breaking

Lay Summary Online camouflage games reveal trichromats are better at finding birds and eggs than simulated dichromats, but dichromats can learn faster. Color vision varies markedly between species, such as between dichromats with 2 different color receptors, and trichromats with 3. Contrary to expectation, trichromats captured the nightjars and eggs faster than simulated dichromats, but simulated dichromats' capture times were affected less by a number of camouflage variables, and they learnt to capture eggs faster. Twitter: @SensoryEcology #colsci

[1]  N. Dominy,et al.  Ecological importance of trichromatic vision to primates , 2001, Nature.

[2]  M. Vorobyev,et al.  Detection of Fruit and the Selection of Primate Visual Pigments for Color Vision , 2004, The American Naturalist.

[3]  Richard A. Robb,et al.  Visualization of patient-specific volume images in image-guided diagnosis and therapy , 2003, J. Vis..

[4]  Martin Stevens,et al.  Revealed by conspicuousness: distractive markings reduce camouflage , 2013 .

[5]  Colour-Blindness and Camouflage , 1940, Nature.

[6]  C. Chubb,et al.  The scaling effects of substrate texture on camouflage patterning in cuttlefish , 2009, Vision Research.

[7]  D. Kline,et al.  Food search through the eyes of a monkey: A functional substitution approach for assessing the ecology of primate color vision , 2013, Vision Research.

[8]  R. Douglas,et al.  Long-wave sensitivity in deep-sea stomiid dragonfish with far-red bioluminescence: evidence for a dietary origin of the chlorophyll-derived retinal photosensitizer of Malacosteus niger. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[9]  R. Maia,et al.  The adaptive value of primate color vision for predator detection , 2014, American journal of primatology.

[10]  A. Stockman,et al.  A luminous efficiency function, V*(lambda), for daylight adaptation. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[11]  Aaron Christ,et al.  Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R , 2009 .

[12]  A. Bond,et al.  Searching image in blue jays: Facilitation and interference in sequential priming , 1999 .

[13]  M. Huynen,et al.  Disruptive coloration and background pattern matching , 2005, Nature.

[14]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision , 2006, Biology Letters.

[15]  T Troscianko,et al.  Stability of the color-opponent signals under changes of illuminant in natural scenes. , 2005, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[16]  M. Stevens,et al.  Pattern mimicry of host eggs by the common cuckoo, as seen through a bird's eye , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  Sami Merilaita,et al.  Defining disruptive coloration and distinguishing its functions , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  Albert Mosyak,et al.  Study of thermal patterns on the heated wall by infra-red technique , 2002 .

[19]  Zygmunt Pizlo,et al.  Camouflage and visual perception , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  G. D. Bernard,et al.  Positive selection of a duplicated UV-sensitive visual pigment coincides with wing pigment evolution in Heliconius butterflies , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  D. Bates,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .

[22]  J. Lythgoe,et al.  Zebra stripes and tiger stripes: the spatial frequency distribution of the pattern compared to that of the background is significant in display and crypsis , 1987 .

[23]  Martin Stevens,et al.  Image calibration and analysis toolbox – a free software suite for objectively measuring reflectance, colour and pattern , 2015, Methods in ecology and evolution.

[24]  R. Bonney,et al.  Next Steps for Citizen Science , 2014, Science.

[25]  N. Caine,et al.  A foraging advantage for dichromatic marmosets (Callithrix geoffroyi) at low light intensity , 2010, Biology Letters.

[26]  M. Vorobyev,et al.  Photoreceptor sectral sensitivities in terrestrial animals: adaptations for luminance and colour vision , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  M. Vorobyev,et al.  Importance of Achromatic Contrast in Short-Range Fruit Foraging of Primates , 2008, PloS one.

[28]  J. Mollon "Tho' she kneel'd in that place where they grew..." The uses and origins of primate colour vision. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[29]  P. King-Smith,et al.  Visual thresholds in dichromats and normals; the importance of Post-receptoral processes , 1981, Vision Research.

[30]  N. Caine,et al.  Demonstration of a foraging advantage for trichromatic marmosets (Callithrix geoffroyi) dependent on food colour , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  N. Mundy,et al.  Trans‐specific evolution of opsin alleles and the maintenance of trichromatic colour vision in Callitrichine primates , 2002, Molecular ecology.

[32]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[33]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Advantages and disadvantages of human dichromacy. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[34]  M. Vorobyev,et al.  Animal colour vision — behavioural tests and physiological concepts , 2003, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[35]  Martin Stevens,et al.  Visual Ecology , 2015 .

[36]  Martin Stevens,et al.  Defeating Crypsis: Detection and Learning of Camouflage Strategies , 2013, PloS one.

[37]  J. Schnoor,et al.  Citizen Science , 2017 .

[38]  C. Hiramatsu,et al.  Polymorphic color vision in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus): Is there foraging niche divergence among phenotypes? , 2008, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[39]  Amanda D. Melin,et al.  Effects of colour vision phenotype on insect capture by a free-ranging population of white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[40]  T. Cronin,et al.  Tuning of photoreceptor spectral sensitivity in fireflies (Coleoptera: Lampyridae) , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[41]  A. Mikami,et al.  Advantage of dichromats over trichromats in discrimination of color‐camouflaged stimuli in nonhuman primates , 2005, American journal of primatology.

[42]  N. Dominy,et al.  Light levels used during feeding by primate species with different color vision phenotypes , 2005, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[43]  M. Stevens,et al.  Camouflage predicts survival in ground-nesting birds , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[44]  J. Mollon,et al.  Dichromats detect colour-camouflaged objects that are not detected by trichromats , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  Toshikazu Hasegawa,et al.  Advantage of Dichromats over Trichromats in Discrimination of Color-Camouflaged Stimuli in Humans , 2006, Perceptual and motor skills.

[46]  D. Osorio,et al.  Effect of colour vision status on insect prey capture efficiency of captive and wild tamarins (Saguinus spp.) , 2012, Animal Behaviour.