The impact of mental workload on inhibitory control subprocesses

The inhibition of inappropriate responses is a function known to rely on prefrontal cortex (PFC) functioning. Similarly, working memory processes are known to rely on the PFC. Even though these processes are usually closely intertwined and the functional neuroanatomy underlying these processes is largely overlapping, the influence of working memory load on inhibitory control process has remained largely elusive. In the current study, we therefore examine how response inhibition processes are modulated by working memory load. For this, we systematically increased the working memory load of participants by integrating mental rotation processes in a Go/NoGo paradigm. To examine the system neurophysiology of these processes in detail, and to examine whether there are differential effects of working memory load on distinct response inhibition subprocesses, we applied event-related potentials (ERPs) in combination with source localization techniques. The data shows that after exceeding a certain threshold, inhibitory control processes are aggravated by working memory load. The neurophysiological data paralleled the behavioral data. However, it suggests that distinguishable response inhibition subprocesses are differentially modulated by working memory load: Changes were evident in the NoGo-P3 amplitude but not in the NoGo-N2 amplitude. On a system level, this distinctive modulation of response inhibition subprocesses was related to differences in neural activity in the left inferior and middle frontal gyri. We show that inhibitory control processes are impaired when the working memory load surpasses a certain threshold. This, however only applies to situations in which the necessity of inhibitory control processes cannot be easily detected on the basis of perceptual factors.

[1]  M. Fuchs,et al.  A standardized boundary element method volume conductor model , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[2]  Laura Busse,et al.  Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  S. Kaufman Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: Can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity? , 2007 .

[4]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  Hugh Garavan,et al.  Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related fMRI study. , 1999 .

[6]  H. Bokura,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[7]  M. Heath,et al.  Visuomotor mental rotation: Reaction time is determined by the complexity of the sensorimotor transformations mediating the response , 2010, Brain Research.

[8]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Hyperpolarization-activated graded persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Christina F. Lavallee,et al.  Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[10]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Neuroimaging Studies of Mental Rotation: A Meta-analysis and Review , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Response inhibition subprocesses and dopaminergic pathways: Basal ganglia disease effects , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Koji Jimura,et al.  Activation of Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus during Response Inhibition across Response Modalities , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  M. Heil,et al.  Toward a chronopsychophysiology of mental rotation. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[15]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[16]  Joo-Seok Hyun,et al.  Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation. , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[17]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[19]  D. Metzler,et al.  Mental rotation: effects of dimensionality of objects and type of task. , 1988, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[20]  Mark S. Cohen,et al.  Changes in cortical activity during mental rotation. A mapping study using functional MRI. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  P L Nunez,et al.  The Spline‐Laplacian in Clinical Neurophysiology: A Method to Improve EEG Spatial Resolution , 1991, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[22]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Sequence effects support the conflict theory of N2 and P3 in the Go/NoGo task. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[23]  H. Heinze,et al.  Cortical Activations during the Mental Rotation of Different Visual Objects , 2001, NeuroImage.

[24]  M. Petrides,et al.  Functional activation of the human brain during mental rotation , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  On the role of fronto-striatal neural synchronization processes for response inhibition—Evidence from ERP phase-synchronization analyses in pre-manifest Huntington's disease gene mutation carriers , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  Stewart H. Mostofsky,et al.  Evidence that response inhibition is a primary deficit in ADHD , 2007, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[27]  G. Band,et al.  Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: the roles of activation and inhibition , 2003, Psychological research.

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control , 2013, Progress in Neurobiology.

[29]  Christian Beste,et al.  Individual differences in ERPs during mental rotation of characters: Lateralization, and performance level , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[30]  G. Band,et al.  Age effects on response monitoring in a mental-rotation task , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[31]  Yohtaro Takano,et al.  Mental Rotation , 2008 .

[32]  Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al.  Suitable stimuli to obtain (no) gender differences in the speed of cognitive processes involved in mental rotation , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[33]  D. Friedman,et al.  The novelty P3: an event-related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain's evaluation of novelty , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[34]  C. Curtis,et al.  Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  T. Braver,et al.  A theory of cognitive control, aging cognition, and neuromodulation , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[36]  C. Beste,et al.  A causal role of the right inferior frontal cortex in implementing strategies for multi-component behaviour , 2015, Nature Communications.

[37]  M. Heil,et al.  Response preparation begins before mental rotation is finished: evidence from event-related brain potentials. , 1998, Acta psychologica.

[38]  Brian Caffo,et al.  Effects of Working Memory Demand on Neural Mechanisms of Motor Response Selection and Control , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Todd C. Handy,et al.  Event-related potentials : a methods handbook , 2005 .

[40]  Josep Marco-Pallarés,et al.  Combined ICA-LORETA analysis of mismatch negativity , 2005, NeuroImage.

[41]  Seth A. Herd,et al.  A Unified Framework for Inhibitory Control Opinion , 2022 .

[42]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  A Developmental Functional MRI Study of Prefrontal Activation during Performance of a Go-No-Go Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  On how high performers keep cool brains in situations of cognitive overload , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[44]  F. Rösler,et al.  On separating processes of event categorization, task preparation, and mental rotation proper in a handedness recognition task. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[45]  C. Beste,et al.  Psychophysiological mechanisms of interindividual differences in goal activation modes during action cascading. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[47]  M. Just,et al.  Interactions between the dorsal and the ventral pathways in mental rotation: An fMRI study , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[48]  F. Collette,et al.  Influence of response prepotency strength, general working memory resources, and specific working memory load on the ability to inhibit predominant responses: A comparison of young and elderly participants , 2011, Brain and Cognition.

[49]  T. Robbins,et al.  Atomoxetine Modulates Right Inferior Frontal Activation During Inhibitory Control: A Pharmacological Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2009, Biological Psychiatry.

[50]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[51]  G. Egan,et al.  Greater superior than inferior parietal lobule activation with increasing rotation angle during mental rotation: An fMRI study , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  J Mazziotta,et al.  A probabilistic atlas and reference system for the human brain: International Consortium for Brain Mapping (ICBM). , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[53]  H. Leung,et al.  Common and Differential Ventrolateral Prefrontal Activity during Inhibition of Hand and Eye Movements , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Kensuke Sekihara,et al.  Localization bias and spatial resolution of adaptive and non-adaptive spatial filters for MEG source reconstruction , 2005, NeuroImage.

[55]  D. Pizzagalli,et al.  When ‘go’ and ‘nogo’ are equally frequent: ERP components and cortical tomography , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[56]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[57]  A. Norcia,et al.  Changes in cortical activity during suppression in stereoblindness , 2000, Neuroreport.

[58]  Martin Heil,et al.  The functional significance of ERP effects during mental rotation. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[59]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Activation of human prefrontal cortex during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks measured by functional MRI. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  John Duncan,et al.  The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control , 2010, NeuroImage.

[61]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[62]  K. Miller Executive functions. , 2005, Pediatric annals.

[63]  Stewart H. Mostofsky,et al.  Increased intra-individual reaction time variability in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across response inhibition tasks with different cognitive demands , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  M. Heath,et al.  The Visuomotor Mental Rotation Task: Visuomotor Transformation Times Are Reduced for Small and Perceptually Familiar Angles , 2011, Journal of motor behavior.

[65]  P. Skudlarski,et al.  Brain Connectivity Related to Working Memory Performance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  J. Pekar,et al.  fMRI evidence that the neural basis of response inhibition is task-dependent. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[67]  A. Baddeley,et al.  The Psychology of Learning and Motivation , 1974 .

[68]  Xiaolin Zhou,et al.  The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation , 2009, Brain Research.

[69]  M. Lenhardt,et al.  High‐dose alcohol intoxication differentially modulates cognitive subprocesses involved in response inhibition , 2016, Addiction biology.

[70]  E. Jodo,et al.  Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[71]  Erica B. Michael,et al.  fMRI investigation of sentence comprehension by eye and by ear: Modality fingerprints on cognitive processes , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[72]  B. Postle,et al.  Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[73]  E. Stein,et al.  Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[74]  Mario Liotti,et al.  Inhibitory control in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: event-related potentials identify the processing component and timing of an impaired right-frontal response-inhibition mechanism , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[75]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[76]  R D Pascual-Marqui,et al.  Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. , 2002, Methods and findings in experimental and clinical pharmacology.

[77]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  Visual working memory as the substrate for mental rotation , 2007 .

[78]  O. Güntürkün,et al.  Response inhibition is modulated by functional cerebral asymmetries for facial expression perception , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[79]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.