Neuroimaging as a Tool for Functionally Decomposing Cognitive Processes

Both those who extol and those who castigate neuroimaging studies and their invocation in cognitive science often misconstrue the contribution neuroimaging is seeking to make, and is capable of making, to cognitive science. We do believe that advances in neuroimaging, including functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), as well as such techniques as single cell recording, are important contributions to the experimental repertoire of cognitive science. We also anticipate that neuroimaging’s importance will increase with improvements in imaging technologies and techniques. Our objective here, however, is neither to extol nor to castigate neuroimaging, but to make clear what sort of contribution neuroimaging, when done well, can make to understanding and explaining mental phenomena. Many of those who adopt extreme views of neuroimaging are not themselves practitioners of the technique and fail to appreciate principles that are commonplace or platitudes among the expert practitioners. Of course, some who adopt extreme views are practitioners. Accordingly, though we are by no means expert practitioners, we start with some commonplaces that are not always transparent in reports of neuroimaging, but are generally understood by the researchers conducting the research (see, for example, Logothetis 2008) and need to be taken into account by those evaluating it:

[1]  Diane Pecher,et al.  Sensorimotor simulations underlie conceptual representations: Modality-specific effects of prior activation , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[2]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[3]  W. Wimsatt Re-Engineering Philosophy for Limited Beings: Piecewise Approximations to Reality , 2007 .

[4]  Lawrence W. Barsalou,et al.  The instability of graded structure: implications for the nature of concepts , 1987 .

[5]  W. Bechtel Mental Mechanisms: Philosophical Perspectives on Cognitive Neuroscience , 2007 .

[6]  Helen E. Fisher,et al.  Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  C. Koch,et al.  Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain , 2005, Nature.

[8]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Diminishing Reciprocal Fairness by Disrupting the Right Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, Science.

[9]  Robert N. McCauley,et al.  Explanatory Pluralism and Heuristic Identity Theory , 2001 .

[10]  R. Boyd,et al.  In Search of Homo Economicus: Behavioral Experiments in 15 Small- Scale Societies , 2001 .

[11]  D. V. Essen,et al.  Neural mechanisms of form and motion processing in the primate visual system , 1994, Neuron.

[12]  A. Cowey,et al.  Striate cortex (V1) activity gates awareness of motion , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The Neural Basis of Economic Decision-Making in the Ultimatum Game , 2003, Science.

[14]  Jelliffe Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde , 1910 .

[15]  N. Logothetis What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI , 2008, Nature.

[16]  D. Keltner,et al.  Love and the commitment problem in romantic relations and friendship. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Carl F Craver,et al.  The Making of a Memory Mechanism , 2003, Journal of the history of biology.

[18]  R. Passingham,et al.  Reading Hidden Intentions in the Human Brain , 2007, Current Biology.

[19]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  G. Rees The new phrenology: the limits of localizing cognitive processes in the brain , 2002 .

[21]  William Bechtel,et al.  Discovering Complexity: Decomposition and Localization as Strategies in Scientific Research , 2010 .

[22]  Robert N. McCauley,et al.  Heuristic Identity Theory (or Back to the Future): The Mind-Body Problem Against the Background of Research Strategies in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2020, Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.

[23]  R. Poldrack Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  K. Yu,et al.  Attentional modulation of sensorimotor processes in the absence of perceptual awareness , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[28]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Perceptual simulation in property verification , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[29]  Robert N. McCauley Reduction: Models of cross-scientific relations and their implications for the psychology-neuroscience interface , 2007 .

[30]  Robert C. Richardson,et al.  Multiple realization and methodological pluralism , 2009, Synthese.

[31]  John R. Anderson How Can the Human Mind Occur in the Physical Universe , 2007 .

[32]  Nikos K Logothetis,et al.  On the nature of the BOLD fMRI contrast mechanism. , 2004, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[33]  W. Uttal The New Phrenology: The Limits of Localizing Cognitive Processes in the Brain , 2001 .

[34]  Richard Henson,et al.  Forward inference using functional neuroimaging: dissociations versus associations , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Christopher L. Asplund,et al.  Isolation of a Central Bottleneck of Information Processing with Time-Resolved fMRI , 2006, Neuron.

[36]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Brain Imaging, Connectionism, and Cognitive Neuropsychology , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[37]  B. Bahrami,et al.  Attentional Load Modulates Responses of Human Primary Visual Cortex to Invisible Stimuli , 2007, Current Biology.