On the Nature of Long-Distance Anaphors

I consider two interpretive properties of long-distance anaphors: (a) they have only a pure-reflexive reading, in the sense that they fail to refer to portrayals of their antecedent, and (b) whenever possible, they must be interpreted as de se, or to put it better, as first-personal. Long-distance anaphors seem to be interpretively parallel to local reflexive construals such as those seen with inherent reflexivity and constructions of inalienable possession. Moreover, in some languages, such as Chinese and Italian, long-distance anaphors can also be locally bound, and in such cases, they pattern with self anaphors rather than with inherent reflexives. To account for this pattern, I propose that long-distance anaphors, together with the local construals mentioned above, are the spell-out of an unsaturated position, saturated in the course of the derivation via -identification with the antecedent.

[1]  C. T. James Huang,et al.  Logophoricity, attitudes, and Ziji at the interface , 2000 .

[2]  Ken Safir,et al.  Implied non-coreference and the pattern of anaphora , 1992 .

[3]  James Higginbotham,et al.  Remembering, Imagining, and the First Person , 2009 .

[4]  H. Castañeda ‘He’: A study in the logic of self-consciousness , 2001 .

[5]  Alessandra Giorgi Long Distance Anaphors and Temporal Relations , 2004 .

[6]  P. Cole Principles and parameters of long-distance reflexives , 1990 .

[7]  E. Reuland Primitives of Binding , 2001, Linguistic Inquiry.

[8]  J.F.A.K. van Benthem,et al.  Semantics and Contextual Expressions , 1989 .

[9]  G. Segal,et al.  Knowledge of Meaning , 2000 .

[10]  Eric Reuland Anaphors, Logophors, And Binding , 2000 .

[11]  K. Safir The Syntax of (In)dependence , 2004 .

[12]  T. Reinhart,et al.  Long-distance anaphora: Anaphors and logophors: an argument structure perspective , 1991 .

[13]  C. Liu Pure Reflexivity, Pure Identity, Focus and Chinese Ziji-Benshen , 2003 .

[14]  M. Everaert,et al.  Toward a More Complete Typology of Anaphoric Expressions , 1999, Linguistic Inquiry.

[15]  Luigi Burzio The morphological basis of anaphora , 1991, Journal of Linguistics.

[16]  Alessandra Giorgi,et al.  Toward a syntax of the subjunctive mood , 2009 .

[17]  Jeffery Lidz,et al.  The Argument Structure Of Verbal Reflexives , 2001 .

[18]  N. Hornstein Movement and Control , 1999, Linguistic Inquiry.

[19]  I. Landau Elements of control , 2000 .

[20]  Howard K. Wettstein,et al.  Themes from Kaplan , 1989 .

[21]  Maria Luisa Zubizarreta,et al.  The definitive determiner and the inalienable constructions in French and in English , 1992 .

[22]  Pierre Pica On the Nature of the Reflexivization Cycle , 1987 .

[23]  E. Williams Thematic structure in syntax , 1994 .

[24]  Eric Reuland,et al.  Long-distance anaphora: Index , 1991 .

[25]  Pierre Pica,et al.  Atomism and Binding , 2000 .

[26]  A. Barber Epistemology of language , 2003 .

[27]  A. Giorgi TOWARD A THEORY OF LONG DISTANCE ANAPHORS: a GB approach , 1984 .

[28]  G. Chierchia,et al.  Anaphora and Attitudes De Se , 1989 .

[29]  Ken Safir,et al.  The Syntax of Anaphora , 2004 .

[30]  Nathan Salmon,et al.  Reflexivity , 1986, Notre Dame J. Formal Log..

[31]  D. Lewis Attitudes De Dicto and De Se , 1979 .

[32]  Gabriella Hermon,et al.  Long-distance reflexives , 2001 .

[33]  R. Jackendoff MME. Tussaud meets the binding theory , 1992 .