Pantomime-Grasping: Advance Knowledge of Haptic Feedback Availability Supports an Absolute Visuo-Haptic Calibration

An emerging issue in movement neurosciences is whether haptic feedback influences the nature of the information supporting a simulated grasping response (i.e., pantomime-grasping). In particular, recent work by our group contrasted pantomime-grasping responses performed with (i.e., PH+ trials) and without (i.e., PH− trials) terminal haptic feedback in separate blocks of trials. Results showed that PH− trials were mediated via relative visual information. In contrast, PH+ trials showed evidence of an absolute visuo-haptic calibration—a finding attributed to an error signal derived from a comparison between expected and actual haptic feedback (i.e., an internal forward model). The present study examined whether advanced knowledge of haptic feedback availability influences the aforementioned calibration process. To that end, PH− and PH+ trials were completed in separate blocks (i.e., the feedback schedule used in our group’s previous study) and a block wherein PH− and PH+ trials were randomly interleaved on a trial-by-trial basis (i.e., random feedback schedule). In other words, the random feedback schedule precluded participants from predicting whether haptic feedback would be available at the movement goal location. We computed just-noticeable-difference (JND) values to determine whether responses adhered to, or violated, the relative psychophysical principles of Weber’s law. Results for the blocked feedback schedule replicated our group’s previous work, whereas in the random feedback schedule PH− and PH+ trials were supported via relative visual information. Accordingly, we propose that a priori knowledge of haptic feedback is necessary to support an absolute visuo-haptic calibration. Moreover, our results demonstrate that the presence and expectancy of haptic feedback is an important consideration in contrasting the behavioral and neural properties of natural and simulated grasping.

[1]  Miles C. Bowman,et al.  Control strategies in object manipulation tasks , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[2]  M. Jeannerod Mechanisms of visuomotor coordination: A study in normal and brain-damaged subjects , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  T. Inui,et al.  How Vision Affects Kinematic Properties of Pantomimed Prehension Movements , 2013, Front. Psychology.

[4]  M. Heath,et al.  Weber’s law in tactile grasping and manual estimation: Feedback-dependent evidence for functionally distinct processing streams , 2014, Brain and Cognition.

[5]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Haptic perception: A tutorial , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[6]  M. Goodale,et al.  Sight Unseen: An Exploration of Conscious and Unconscious Vision , 2004 .

[7]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Utilization of Visual Feedback Information during Rapid Pointing Movements , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[8]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  D. Westwood,et al.  Pantomime-grasping: the ‘return’ of haptic feedback supports the absolute specification of object size , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Mark Mon-Williams,et al.  Natural prehension in trials without haptic feedback but only when calibration is allowed , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  E. Brenner,et al.  A new view on grasping. , 1999, Motor control.

[12]  Charles E. Pettypiece,et al.  Differential effects of delay upon visually and haptically guided grasping and perceptual judgments , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Calibration of visually guided reaching is driven by error-corrective learning and internal dynamics. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[16]  R. Johansson,et al.  Responses in glabrous skin mechanoreceptors during precision grip in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  T. Ganel,et al.  Response: When does grasping escape Weber's law? , 2008, Current Biology.

[18]  M. Lemay,et al.  A distance effect in a manual aiming task to remembered targets: a test of three hypotheses , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Memory-Guided Reaching: What the Visuomotor System Knows and How Long It Knows It , 2010 .

[20]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Grasping time does not influence the early adherence of aperture shaping to Weber's law , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[21]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[22]  M. Heath,et al.  Visually and memory-guided grasping: Aperture shaping exhibits a time-dependent scaling to Weber’s law , 2011, Vision Research.

[23]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[24]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  Tzvi Ganel,et al.  Visual coding for action violates fundamental psychophysical principles , 2008, Current Biology.

[26]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Vision and Goal-Directed Movement: Neurobehavioral Perspectives , 2010 .

[27]  Steve Hansen,et al.  Optimising speed and energy expenditure in accurate visually directed upper limb movements , 2009, Ergonomics.

[28]  M. Heath,et al.  Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  David A. Winter,et al.  Signal processing and linear systems for the movement sciences , 1997 .

[30]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Optimal Control Strategies Under Different Feedback Schedules: Kinematic Evidence , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  J. D. Connolly,et al.  Optic ataxia as a model to investigate the role of the posterior parietal cortex in visually guided action: evidence from studies of patient M.H. , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[32]  M. Heath,et al.  An Inverse Grip Starting Posture Gives Rise to Time-Dependent Adherence to Weber's Law: A Reply to Ganel et al. (2014). , 2015, Journal of Vision.

[33]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Goal-directed reaching: movement strategies influence the weighting of allocentric and egocentric visual cues , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Thomas Schenk,et al.  No Dissociation between Perception and Action in Patient DF When Haptic Feedback is Withdrawn , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  A. Milner,et al.  The Magic Grasp: Motor Expertise in Deception , 2011, PloS one.

[36]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Does grasping in patient D.F. depend on vision? , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  E. Kaplan,et al.  A human cerebral deconnection syndrome , 1962, Neurology.

[38]  M. Goodale,et al.  Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: an fMRI study. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[40]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Optic Ataxia: From Balint’s Syndrome to the Parietal Reach Region , 2014, Neuron.

[41]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[42]  M. Goodale Transforming vision into action , 2011, Vision Research.

[43]  L. Marks,et al.  Chapter 2 – Psychophysical Scaling , 1998 .

[44]  Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al.  Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[45]  N. Geschwind,et al.  A Human Cerebral Deconnection Syndrome , 1974 .

[46]  Stephanie Hosang,et al.  Grasping a 2D object: terminal haptic feedback supports an absolute visuo-haptic calibration , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  G. Cumming Understanding the New Statistics: Effect Sizes, Confidence Intervals, and Meta-Analysis , 2011 .

[48]  M. Heath,et al.  Goal-directed grasping: The dimensional properties of an object influence the nature of the visual information mediating aperture shaping , 2013, Brain and Cognition.

[49]  M. Heath,et al.  Distinct Visual Cues Mediate Aperture Shaping for Grasping and Pantomime-Grasping Tasks , 2013, Journal of motor behavior.

[50]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The visual coding of grip aperture shows an early but not late adherence to Weber's law , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[51]  Paul Milgram,et al.  A spectacle-mounted liquid-crystal tachistoscope , 1987 .

[52]  G. Buckingham,et al.  Reframing the action and perception dissociation in DF: haptics matters, but how? , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  L. Jakobson,et al.  Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  张静,et al.  Banana Ovate family protein MaOFP1 and MADS-box protein MuMADS1 antagonistically regulated banana fruit ripening , 2015 .

[56]  D. Westwood,et al.  Task Demands and Limb Apraxia in Stroke , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[57]  Erez Freud,et al.  Accurate Visuomotor Control below the Perceptual Threshold of Size Discrimination , 2012, PloS one.

[58]  David A. Westwood,et al.  Pantomimed actions may be controlled by the ventral visual stream , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[59]  E. Pedhazur Multiple Regression in Behavioral Research: Explanation and Prediction , 1982 .

[60]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  DF's visual brain in action: The role of tactile cues , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  Stephanie Hosang,et al.  Memory delay and haptic feedback influence the dissociation of tactile cues for perception and action , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[62]  O. Krigolson,et al.  Event-related brain potentials during the visuomotor mental rotation task: The contingent negative variation scales to angle of rotation , 2015, Neuroscience.