Self-Affirmation Reduces Biased Processing of Health-Risk Information

An experiment tested whether a positive experience (the endorsement and recall of one's past acts of kindness) would reduce biased processing of self-relevant health-risk information. Women college students (N = 66) who reported high or low levels of daily caffeine use were exposed to both risk-confirming and risk-disconfirming information about the link between caffeine consumption and fibrocystic breast disease (FBD). Participants were randomly assigned to complete an affirmation of their kindness via questionnaire or to a no-affirmation condition. Results indicated that the affirmation manipulation made frequent caffeine drinkers more open, less biased processors of risk-related information. Relative to frequent caffeine drinkers who did not affirm their kindness, frequent caffeine drinkers in the affirmation condition oriented more quickly to the risk-confirming information, rated the risk-confirming information as more convincing than the risk-disconfirming information, and recalled less risk-disconfirming information at a 1-week follow-up. They also reported greater perceived personal control over reducing their level of caffeine consumption. Although frequent caffeine drinkers in the affirmation condition initially reported lower intentions to reduce their caffeine consumption, there was no evidence that they were less likely to decrease their caffeine consumption at the follow-up. The possibility that positive beliefs and experiences function as self-regulatory resources among people confronting threats to health and well-being is discussed.

[1]  Robert S. Wyer,et al.  The effects of feedback about self and others on the recall and judgments of feedback-relevant information ☆ , 1983 .

[2]  A. Greenwald The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. , 1980 .

[3]  R. Baumeister The Optimal Margin of Illusion , 1989 .

[4]  Claude M. Steele,et al.  DISSONANCE PROCESSES AS SELF-AFFIRMATION , 1983 .

[5]  A. O'Leary,et al.  Self-efficacy and health. , 1985, Behaviour research and therapy.

[6]  J. Beckmann,et al.  Action control : from cognition to behavior , 1985 .

[7]  W. Klein,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Resistance of Personal Risk Perceptions to Debiasing Interventions , 2002 .

[8]  C. la Vecchia,et al.  Benign breast disease and consumption of beverages containing methylxanthines. , 1985, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[9]  J. Kelsey,et al.  Caffeine consumption and fibrocystic breast disease: a case-control epidemiologic study. , 1984, Journal of the National Cancer Institute.

[10]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The psychology of attitudes. , 1993 .

[11]  Lisa G. Aspinwall,et al.  Distinguishing Optimism from Denial: Optimistic Beliefs Predict Attention to Health Threats , 1996 .

[12]  C. Steele The Psychology of Self-Affirmation: Sustaining the Integrity of the Self , 1988 .

[13]  Icek Ajzen,et al.  From Intentions to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior , 1985 .

[14]  M. Strube,et al.  Self-Evaluation: To Thine Own Self Be Good, To Thine Own Self Be Sure, To Thine Own Self Be True, an , 1997 .

[15]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behavior , 1991 .

[16]  A. Isen,et al.  Success, failure, attention, and reaction to others: The warm glow of success. , 1970 .

[17]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Maintaining Positive Illusions in the Face of Negative Information: Getting the Facts Without Letting them Get to You , 1989 .

[18]  J. Block,et al.  Do positive illusions foster mental health? An examination of the Taylor and Brown formulation. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[19]  B. Fredrickson What Good Are Positive Emotions? , 1998, Review of general psychology : journal of Division 1, of the American Psychological Association.

[20]  L. Aspinwall Rethinking the Role of Positive Affect in Self-Regulation , 1998 .

[21]  J. Darley,et al.  Appraising the threat of illness: a mental representational approach. , 1988, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[22]  J. Suls,et al.  Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research. , 1991 .

[23]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[24]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Prediction of goal directed behaviour: Attitudes, intentions and perceived behavioural control , 1986 .

[25]  Y. Trope,et al.  Reconciling competing motives in self-evaluation: the role of self-control in feedback seeking. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  Y. Trope,et al.  Resolving Conflicts Among Self-Evaluative Motives: Positive Experiences as a Resource for Overcoming Defensiveness , 1998 .

[27]  Peter H. Ditto,et al.  Motivated Skepticism: Use of Differential Decision Criteria for Preferred and Nonpreferred Conclusions , 1992 .

[28]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Chronic issue-specific fear inhibits systematic processing of persuasive communications. , 1990 .

[29]  L. Festinger,et al.  A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance , 2017 .

[30]  A. Tesser Reproductions Supplied by Edrs Are the Best That Can Be Made * * from the Original Document. toward a Self-evaluation Maintenance Model of Social Behavior* , 2007 .

[31]  Jonathon D. Brown,et al.  Evaluating one's abilities: Shortcuts and stumbling blocks on the road to self-knowledge , 1990 .

[32]  James A. Scepansky,et al.  Motivated sensitivity to preference-inconsistent information. , 1998 .

[33]  George W. Albee,et al.  Primary Prevention of AIDS: Psychological Approaches , 1989 .

[34]  A. Isen,et al.  Positive affect and decision making. , 1993 .

[35]  S. Hobfoll Conservation of resources. A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[36]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Self-Evaluation Processes , 1995 .

[37]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Psychosocial aspects of chronic illness. , 1996 .

[38]  C. Steele,et al.  Attributional analysis as self-affirmation. , 1986 .

[39]  A. Tesser,et al.  On the confluence of self processes , 1991 .

[40]  R. Croyle Psychosocial effects of screening for disease prevention and detection , 1995 .

[41]  William B. Swann,et al.  The Search for Beauty and Truth: A Framework for Understanding Reactions to Evaluations , 1995 .

[42]  W. Klein,et al.  Social comparison and unrealistic optimism about personal risk. , 1997 .

[43]  Z. Kunda,et al.  The case for motivated reasoning. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.

[44]  J. Bargh,et al.  The psychology of action : linking cognition and motivation to behavior , 1999 .

[45]  Dieter Frey,et al.  Recent Research on Selective Exposure to Information , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[46]  Claude M. Steele,et al.  Name-calling and compliance. , 1975 .

[47]  Mahzarin R. Banaji,et al.  The Self in Social Contexts , 1994 .

[48]  Y. Trope Self-assessment, self-enhancement, and task preference. , 1980 .

[49]  Lisa G. Aspinwall,et al.  Mediating and moderating processes in psychosocial stress: Appraisal, coping, resistance, and vulnerability. , 1996 .

[50]  Karin S. Frey,et al.  Changing patterns of comparative behavior as skills are acquired: A functional model of self-evaluation. , 1991 .

[51]  M. Zuckerman,et al.  Determinants of information-seeking behavior , 1979 .

[52]  Howard B. Kaplan,et al.  Psychosocial Stress: Perspectives on Structure, Theory, Life-Course, and Methods , 1997 .

[53]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[54]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research , 1977 .

[55]  R. Croyle,et al.  Judging health status: effects of perceived prevalence and personal relevance. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  John R. Aiello,et al.  Self-consistency, self-enhancement, and accuracy in reactions to feedback , 1995 .

[57]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Positive illusions and well-being revisited: separating fact from fiction. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[58]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Selective Use of Heunrstic and Systematic Processing Under Defense Motivation , 1997 .

[59]  J. Greenberg,et al.  Toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypothesis-testing model , 1987 .

[60]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. , 1987 .

[61]  A. Bandura Perceived self-efficacy in the exercise of control over AIDS infection , 1990 .

[62]  M. Strube,et al.  The Multiply Motivated Self , 1995 .

[63]  H. Leventhal,et al.  Findings and Theory in the Study of Fear Communications , 1970 .

[64]  G. Keppel Design and analysis: A researcher's handbook, 3rd ed. , 1991 .

[65]  J. V. Wood,et al.  Theory and Research Concerning Social Comparisons of Personal Attributes , 1989 .

[66]  G. Keppel,et al.  Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook , 1976 .

[67]  Ralf Schwarzer,et al.  Optimism, Vulnerability, and self-beliefs as health-related cognitions: A systematic overview , 1994 .

[68]  Bram P. Buunk,et al.  Health, Coping, and Well-being : Perspectives From Social Comparison Theory , 1997 .

[69]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  A stitch in time: self-regulation and proactive coping. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[70]  A. Isen,et al.  Positive Affect Facilitates Integration of Information and Decreases Anchoring in Reasoning among Physicians , 1997 .

[71]  A. Tesser,et al.  On the substitutability of self-protective mechanisms , 1996 .

[72]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  Defensive Processing of Personally Relevant Health Messages , 1992 .

[73]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Difficulty and diagnosticity as determinants of choice among tasks. , 1975 .

[74]  P. R. Sachs Avoidance of Diagnostic Information in Self-Evaluation of Ability , 1982 .

[75]  C M Steele,et al.  Self-image resilience and dissonance: the role of affirmational resources. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[76]  Claude M. Steele,et al.  Making the Dissonant Act Unreflective of Self , 1981 .

[77]  J. Cooper,et al.  A New Look at Dissonance Theory , 1984 .

[78]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Situated Optimism: Specific Outcome Expectancies and Self-Regulation , 1998 .

[79]  P. Ditto,et al.  Perceived Accuracy of Favorable and Unfavorable Psychological Feedback , 1995 .

[80]  R. P. Fishburne,et al.  Derivation of New Readability Formulas (Automated Readability Index, Fog Count and Flesch Reading Ease Formula) for Navy Enlisted Personnel , 1975 .

[81]  L. Aspinwall Future-oriented aspects of social comparisons: A framework for studying health-related comparison activity. , 1997 .

[82]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  Taking a Closer Look: Reasserting the Role of the Self-Concept in Dissonance Theory , 1992 .

[83]  P. Tannenbaum,et al.  Theories of cognitive consistency: a sourcebook. , 1968 .

[84]  Elliot Aronson,et al.  The Theory of Cognitive Dissonance: A Current Perspective1 , 1969 .

[85]  Jonathon D. Brown,et al.  Truth and Consequences: The Costs and Benefits of Accurate Self-Knowledge , 1995 .