Transfer of time-based task expectancy across different timing environments
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] U. Hess,et al. Transformations for within-subject designs: a Monte Carlo investigation. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.
[2] P. Coignet. On specificity , 2003 .
[3] Joachim Hoffmann,et al. Temporal cueing of target-identity and target-location. , 2010, Experimental psychology.
[4] Gesine Dreisbach,et al. Time-based event expectations employ relative, not absolute, representations of time , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] Robert Langner,et al. Distraction by irrelevant sound during foreperiods selectively impairs temporal preparation. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[6] G. Dreisbach,et al. The time-event correlation effect is due to temporal expectancy, not to partial transition costs. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] David E Huber,et al. Implicit learning of spatiotemporal contingencies in spatial cueing. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Task predictability influences the variable foreperiod effect: evidence of task-specific temporal preparation , 2015, Psychological research.
[9] A. Machado. Learning the temporal dynamics of behavior. , 1997, Psychological review.
[10] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Sequential effects within a short foreperiod context: evidence for the conditioning account of temporal preparation. , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[11] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[12] D. Donaldson,et al. Listening to the sound of silence: disfluent silent pauses in speech have consequences for listeners , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[13] Wouter Kruijne,et al. Hazard Versus History: Temporal Preparation Is Driven by Past Experience , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] Wouter Kruijne,et al. Outlines of a multiple trace theory of temporal preparation , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[15] Hans Colonius,et al. Advances in modern mental chronometry , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[16] J. E. Weaver,et al. Pigeons group time intervals according to their relative duration , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[17] Hugo Merchant,et al. Neurobiology of Interval Timing , 2014, Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology.
[18] Risto Näätänen,et al. Time uncertainty and occurence uncertainty of the stimulus in a simple reaction time task , 1972 .
[19] R. Block,et al. Time perception, attention, and memory: a selective review. , 2014, Acta psychologica.
[20] Simon Grondin,et al. Timing and time perception: A selective review and commentary on recent reviews , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[21] Robert Langner,et al. Arousal modulates temporal preparation under increased time uncertainty: Evidence from higher-order sequential foreperiod effects. , 2012, Acta psychologica.
[22] Joachim Hoffmann,et al. The specificity of temporal expectancy: Evidence from a variable foreperiod paradigm , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[23] J. Hoffmann,et al. Time-Based Expectancy for Task Relevant Stimulus Features , 2016 .
[24] Mike Wendt,et al. Conflict adaptation in time: Foreperiods as contextual cues for attentional adjustment , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[25] Joachim Hoffmann,et al. Response specific temporal expectancy: Evidence from a variable foreperiod paradigm , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[26] G. Dreisbach,et al. Temporal Predictability Facilitates Action, Not Perception , 2013, Psychological science.
[27] Keikichi Hirose,et al. Filled pauses as cues to the complexity of upcoming phrases for native and non-native listeners , 2008, Speech Commun..
[28] Felicia Roberts,et al. Judgments Concerning the Valence of Inter-Turn Silence Across Speakers of American English, Italian, and Japanese , 2011 .
[29] T. Zentall,et al. Relative judgments affect assessments of stimulus duration , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[30] Andrea M Philipp,et al. Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.
[31] Frank Agter,et al. Reweighting sequential effects across different distributions of foreperiods: Segregating elementary contributions to nonspecific preparation , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[32] David R. Hunter,et al. Aviation Psychology and Human Factors , 2009 .
[33] Gregor Volberg,et al. Time-based expectations entail preparatory motor activity , 2017, Cortex.
[34] Michael Hildebrandt,et al. Detecting system failures from durations and binary cues , 2012, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..
[35] Florian Weber,et al. Improving the Human–Computer Dialogue With Increased Temporal Predictability , 2013, Hum. Factors.
[36] D. Knol,et al. The foreperiod effect revisited: conditioning as a basis for nonspecific preparation. , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[37] Nachshon Meiran,et al. Nonintentional task set activation: Evidence from implicit task sequence learning , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[38] M. Wittmann. The inner sense of time: how the brain creates a representation of duration , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[39] Rolf Ulrich,et al. The effect of a cross-trial shift of auditory warning signals on the sequential foreperiod effect. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[40] Dorit Wenke,et al. Time-Based Expectancy in Temporally Structured Task Switching , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] Alexander L. Francis,et al. Identifying a temporal threshold of tolerance for silent gaps after requests. , 2013, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[42] E. T. Klemmer. Time uncertainty in simple reaction time. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.
[43] Roland Thomaschke,et al. Predictivity of system delays shortens human response time , 2014, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..