Critical Roles for Distance, Task, and Motion in Space Perception: Initial Conceptual Framework and Practical Implications
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] A. S. Gilinsky. Perceived size and distance in visual space. , 1951, Psychological review.
[2] F. Previc. The neuropsychology of 3-D space. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.
[3] B. Bridgeman,et al. Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] F. Ferlazzo,et al. Opposite visual field asymmetries for egocentric and allocentric spatial judgments , 2004, Neuroreport.
[5] J. Marshall,et al. Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.
[6] L. Jakobson,et al. A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.
[7] A. Berti,et al. When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] J. Yolton. Reasons for Realism. Selected Essays of James J. Gibson. Edited by EDWARD REED and REBECCA JONES. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1982. Pp. xvi + 449. $39.95 , 1984 .
[9] David N. Lee,et al. A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.
[10] B. Bridgeman,et al. Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[12] C. Trevarthen,et al. Two mechanisms of vision in primates , 1968, Psychologische Forschung.
[13] Patricia R. DeLucia,et al. The Aperture Illusion can Occur with “3-D” Displays and Active Control: Implications for Minimally-Invasive Surgery , 2006 .
[14] J. Gibson. Observations on active touch. , 1962, Psychological review.
[15] J. Cutting,et al. Minimodularity and the perception of layout. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[16] Patricia R. DeLucia,et al. Chapter 11 Multiple sources of information influence time-to-contact judgments: Do heuristics accommodate limits in sensory and cognitive processes? , 2004 .
[17] Sunanda Mitra,et al. Toward the Improvement of Image-Guided Interventions for Minimally Invasive Surgery: Three Factors That Affect Performance , 2006, Hum. Factors.
[18] M. Goodale,et al. Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.
[19] P R DeLucia,et al. Pictorial and motion-based information for depth perception. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action for perception: a motor-visual attentional effect. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[21] Leslie G. Ungerleider. Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .
[22] Zhonghai Li,et al. An empirical investigation of a dynamic brake light concept for reduction of rear-end collisions through manipulation of optical looming , 2008, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..
[23] Nicola Smania,et al. Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients , 2001, NeuroImage.
[24] Patricia R. DeLucia,et al. Visual Information in Judgments of Head-On Collisions , 2007 .
[25] L. Jakobson,et al. Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[26] J. Norman. Are the direct and indirect theories of perception incompatible? , 1983, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[27] K. Zilles,et al. Neural consequences of acting in near versus far space: a physiological basis for clinical dissociations. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[28] Scott M. Dittman,et al. Monocular optical constraints on collision control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] J. Aloimonos,et al. On the kinetic depth effect , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.
[30] E. Reed,et al. Reasons For Realism: Selected Essays Of James J. Gibson , 1982 .
[31] Fred H. Previc,et al. The Neuropsychology of 3-D Space , 1998 .
[32] I. Gordon. Theories of Visual Perception , 1989 .
[33] S Krafczyk,et al. OBJECT‐MOTION DETECTION AFFECTED BY CONCURRENT SELF‐MOTION PERCEPTION: APPLIED ASPECTS FOR VEHICLE GUIDANCE , 1987, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.
[34] G J Andersen,et al. Active control in interrupted dynamic spatial orientation: The detection of orientation change , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[35] J. Tresilian,et al. Perceptual and cognitive processes in time-to-contact estimation: Analysis of prediction-motion and relative judgment tasks , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] P J Stappers,et al. Forms Can Be Recognized from Dynamic Occlusion Alone , 1989, Perceptual and motor skills.
[37] Bruce Bridgeman,et al. The induced Roelofs effect: two visual systems or the shift of a single reference frame? , 2004, Vision Research.
[38] M. Lévesque. Perception , 1986, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.
[39] W Schiff,et al. Information Used in Judging Impending Collision , 1979, Perception.
[40] Patricia R. DeLucia,et al. Time-to-Collision Judgments of Constant and Non-Constant Velocities: Implications for Rear-End Collisions , 2006 .
[41] Patricia R. DeLucia. Effective information for TTC judgments varies during an approach event , 2010 .
[42] M. Goodale,et al. Active manual control of object views facilitates visual recognition , 1999, Current Biology.
[43] Gereon R. Fink,et al. Spatial Cognition: Where We Were and Where We Are , 2001, NeuroImage.
[44] P R DeLucia,et al. Cognitive motion extrapolation and cognitive clocking in prediction motion task. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[45] M. Alexander,et al. Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .
[46] S. Schultz. Principles of Neural Science, 4th ed. , 2001 .
[47] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[48] P R DeLucia,et al. Pictorial and motion-based depth information during active control of self-motion: size-arrival effects on collision avoidance. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] D. Regan,et al. Simulated self-motion alters perceived time to collision , 2000, Current Biology.
[50] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[51] James E. Cutting,et al. Chapter 3 – Perceiving Layout and Knowing Distances: The Integration, Relative Potency, and Contextual Use of Different Information about Depth* , 1995 .
[52] M. Goodale,et al. Sight Unseen: An Exploration of Conscious and Unconscious Vision , 2004 .
[53] Patricia R. DeLucia,et al. Effects of Optical Flow and Discrete Warnings on Deceleration Detection during Car Following , 2005 .
[54] Julian Hochberg,et al. Representation of motion and space in video and cinematic displays , 1986 .
[55] Beryl Hesketh,et al. A Comparison of Time Estimations in Driving With Target-Only in Motion, Self-Only in Motion, and Self-and-Target in Motion , 2002 .
[56] N. Anderson. Chapter 8 – ALGEBRAIC MODELS IN PERCEPTION* , 1974 .
[57] Qingfeng Huang,et al. An adaptive peer-to-peer collision warning system , 2002, Vehicular Technology Conference. IEEE 55th Vehicular Technology Conference. VTC Spring 2002 (Cat. No.02CH37367).