Cracking the orthographic code: An introduction

In this introduction to the special issue, I will first briefly summarise past research on orthographic processing, describing some of the central areas of empirical investigation and the major theories guiding that work. Next, I will describe the more recent lines of empirical and theoretical research that have emerged over the last decade or so, and that serve as the focus of the current special issue. I will attempt to summarise the key evidence that has emerged from this research, and examine how this evidence can guide model selection. Finally, I will discuss some possible avenues for future research on orthographic processing in the hope of finally ‘cracking the orthographic code’.

[1]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  Sharon M. Thomas,et al.  Assessing the importance of letter pairs in initial, exterior, and interior positions in reading. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[3]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Theoretical analysis of interhemispheric transfer costs in visual word recognition , 2008 .

[4]  C. J. Álvarez,et al.  Syllable Frequency and Visual Word Recognition in Spanish , 1993 .

[5]  R. Treiman,et al.  Are there onset- and rime-like units in printed words? , 1987 .

[6]  C. Davis,et al.  More words in the neighborhood: Interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[7]  T. R. Jordan,et al.  Position-specific masking and the word-letter phenomenon: Reexamining the evidence from the Reicher-Wheeler paradigm. , 1996 .

[8]  G. M. Reicher Perceptual recognition as a function of meaninfulness of stimulus material. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Pseudoword context effects on letter perception: The role of word misperception , 2005 .

[10]  K. Forster,et al.  Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. , 1976 .

[11]  Arnaud Rey,et al.  Graphemic complexity and multiple print-to-sound associations in visual word recognition , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[12]  K I Forster,et al.  The Pros and Cons of Masked Priming , 1998, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[13]  K. Rayner Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Effects of Phonological and Orthographic Neighbourhood Density Interact in Visual Word Recognition , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[15]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Current issues in morphological processing: An introduction , 2005 .

[16]  R. Treiman,et al.  How to see a reading unit , 1986 .

[17]  R W Proctor,et al.  Order-relevant and order-irrelevant decision rules in multiletter matching. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  J. Grainger,et al.  Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: effects of relative prime-target frequency. , 1990 .

[19]  J. Grainger,et al.  Effects of orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition: cross-task comparisons. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[20]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A Theory of Human Memory: Self-Organization and Performance of Sensory-Motor Codes, Maps, and Plans , 1982 .

[21]  Wayne A. Wickelgran Context-sensitive coding, associative memory, and serial order in (speech) behavior. , 1969 .

[22]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. , 2007, Psychological review.

[23]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Letter visibility and the viewing position effect in visual word recognition , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Windows executable software for the progressive demasking task , 2008, Behavior Research Methods.

[25]  J. M. Cattell THE TIME IT TAKES TO SEE AND NAME OBJECTS , 1886 .

[26]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  On the Time Course of Visual Word Recognition: An Event-related Potential Investigation using Masked Repetition Priming , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Brysbaert,et al.  Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths , 2000, Psychological research.

[28]  Michael Garman,et al.  Psycholinguistics: Accessing the mental lexicon , 1990 .

[29]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Masked cross-modal repetition priming: An event-related potential investigation , 2007, Language and cognitive processes.

[30]  J B Poline,et al.  Letter Binding and Invariant Recognition of Masked Words , 2004, Psychological science.

[31]  Kristi A. Kiyonaga,et al.  The time course of orthographic and phonological code activation. , 1993, Psychological science.

[32]  J. Grainger,et al.  Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[33]  A Pollatsek,et al.  The effects of "neighborhood size" in reading and lexical decision. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .

[35]  K. Rayner,et al.  The psychology of reading , 1989 .

[36]  M. Carreiras,et al.  Do transposed-letter similarity effects occur at a morpheme level? Evidence for morpho-orthographic decomposition , 2007, Cognition.

[37]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Do orthotactics and phonology constrain the transposed-letter effect? , 2008 .

[38]  J. Grainger,et al.  Masked cross-modal morphological priming: Unravelling morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic influences in early word recognition , 2005 .

[39]  Timothy R. Jordan,et al.  Perceiving exterior letters of words: differential influences of letter-fragment and non-letter-fragment masks. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  A. Marcel Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[41]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.

[42]  Kevin Diependaele,et al.  Masked repetition and phonological priming within and across modalities. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  R. Holloway The broth in my brother ’ s brothel : Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition , 2005 .

[44]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  A study of relative-position priming with superset primes. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  A. Jacobs,et al.  On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: The neighborhood frequency effect , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[46]  Philip T Quinlan,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word identification , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[47]  J Grainger,et al.  Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.

[48]  M. Mozer,et al.  Perceptual interactions in two-word displays: familiarity and similarity effects. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  J Grainger,et al.  Effects of Orthography are Independent of Phonology in Masked form Priming , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[50]  S. Andrews The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts , 1997 .

[51]  Alexander Pollatsek,et al.  Lexical Access via an Orthographic Code? The Basic Orthographic Syllabic Structure (BOSS) Reconsidered. , 1983 .

[52]  J Grainger,et al.  Neighborhood frequency effects in visual word recognition: A comparison of lexical decision and masked identification latencies , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[53]  K. Forster,et al.  REPETITION PRIMING AND FREQUENCY ATTENUATION IN LEXICAL ACCESS , 1984 .

[54]  C. Whitney How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  John J. L. Morton,et al.  Interaction of information in word recognition. , 1969 .

[56]  K. Rayner The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[57]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  A dual read-out model of word context effects in letter perception: Further investigations of the word superiority effect. , 1994 .

[58]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Graded effects of number of inserted letters in superset priming. , 2008, Experimental psychology.

[59]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Phonology as the source of syllable frequency effects in visual word recognition: Evidence from French , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[60]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[62]  Valerie A. Carr,et al.  Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Modality-Specific and Supramodal Word Processing , 2003, Neuron.

[63]  M Coltheart,et al.  DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.

[64]  Neighborhood frequency does affect performance in the Reicher task: encoding or decision? , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[66]  J. Grainger,et al.  Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: effects of relative prime-target frequency. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[67]  S. Lupker,et al.  Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[68]  Jerome S. Bruner,et al.  A Note on the Informativeness of Parts of Words , 1958 .

[69]  W. Estes,et al.  Discriminability and bias in the word-superiority effect , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[70]  Ram Frost,et al.  Cambridge University versus Hebrew University: The impact of letter transposition on reading English and Hebrew , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[71]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Does “whole-word shape” play a role in visual word recognition? , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[72]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[73]  L. E. Krueger A Theory of Perceptual Matching , 1978 .

[74]  J Grainger,et al.  Masked partial-word priming in visual word recognition: effects of positional letter frequency. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[75]  K. Forster,et al.  Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? , 1987 .

[76]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The structure of graphemic representations , 1990, Cognition.

[77]  S. Liversedge,et al.  Neighborhood Effects Using a Partial Priming Methodology: Guessing or Activation? , 1998 .

[78]  J. Grainger,et al.  Phonology and Orthography in Visual Word Recognition: Evidence from Masked Non-Word Priming , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[79]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Phonological Information Provides Early Sources of Constraint in the Processing of Letter Strings , 1995 .

[80]  K. Forster,et al.  Criterion bias and search sequence bias in word recognition , 1981, Memory & cognition.

[81]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Letter position information and printed word perception: the relative-position priming constraint. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[82]  R Frost,et al.  Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: true issues and false trails. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.

[83]  S. Lupker,et al.  Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions ☆ , 2004 .

[84]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Optimal viewing position effect in word recognition: A challenge to current theory. , 1992 .

[85]  Yasushi Hino,et al.  Neighborhood Size and Neighborhood Frequency Effects in Word Recognition , 1995 .

[86]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  What do letter migration errors reveal about letter position coding in visual word recognition? , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[87]  Ton Dijkstra,et al.  Shared neighborhood effects in masked orthographic priming , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[88]  J. Ziegler,et al.  Visual and Phonological Codes in Letter and Word Recognition: Evidence from Incremental Priming , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[89]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Neural constraints on a functional architecture for word recognition. , 2008 .

[90]  J. Grainger,et al.  Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters , 2004 .

[91]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Transposed-letter effects in reading: evidence from eye movements and parafoveal preview. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[92]  Jeffrey R. Binder,et al.  Tuning of the human left fusiform gyrus to sublexical orthographic structure , 2006, NeuroImage.

[93]  Conrad Perry,et al.  No more problems in Coltheart's neighborhood: resolving neighborhood conflicts in the lexical decision task , 1998, Cognition.

[94]  J Grainger,et al.  The role of letter identity and letter position in orthographic priming , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[95]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Spatial representation of words in the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient , 1990, Nature.

[96]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Memory and cognition , 1977 .

[97]  J. Segui,et al.  Morphological priming without morphological relationship , 2003 .

[98]  S. M. Chambers Letter and order information in lexical access , 1979 .

[99]  S. Andrews Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability , 1996 .

[100]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Perception of multiple objects - a connectionist approach , 1991, Neural network modeling and connectionism.

[101]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The Use of Abstract Graphemic Information in Lexical Access , 1981 .

[102]  Sally Andrews,et al.  Interactive Activation Accounts of Morphological Decomposition: Finding the Trap in Mousetrap? , 1999, Brain and Language.

[103]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Cross-code consistency effects in visual word recognition. , 2008 .

[104]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception , 2004 .

[105]  Manuel Perea,et al.  The frequency effect for pseudowords in the lexical decision task , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[106]  Johannes C. Ziegler,et al.  A developmental perspective on the neural code for written words , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[107]  H. Bouma Visual interference in the parafoveal recognition of initial and final letters of words. , 1973, Vision research.

[108]  Keith Rayner,et al.  Letter transpositions within and across morphemes. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[109]  A. W. Ellis Reading, Writing and Dyslexia: A Cognitive Analysis , 1984 .

[110]  Arnaud Rey,et al.  Graphemes are perceptual reading units , 2000, Cognition.

[111]  M. Carreiras,et al.  Effects of Syllable Frequency and Syllable Neighborhood Frequency in Visual Word Recognition , 1998 .

[112]  D. D. Wheeler Processes in word recognition , 1970 .

[113]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[114]  Greg B. Simpson,et al.  The influence of phonological neighborhood on visual word perception , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[115]  Mozer,et al.  Early parallel processing in reading: a connectionist approach. Technical report, April-November 1986 , 1986 .

[116]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  Contrasting five different theories of letter position coding: evidence from orthographic similarity effects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[117]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Effects of stimulus font and size on masked repetition priming: An event-related potentials (ERP) investigation , 2008, Language and cognitive processes.

[118]  K I Forster,et al.  No enemies in the neighborhood: absence of inhibitory neighborhood effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[119]  G. V. van Orden A ROWS is a ROSE: spelling, sound, and reading. , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[120]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  Visual constraints in written word recognition: evidence from the optimal viewing-position effect , 2005 .

[121]  Morten L. Kringelbach,et al.  Visual word recognition: the first half second , 2004, NeuroImage.

[122]  Manuel Perea,et al.  The overlap model: a model of letter position coding. , 2008, Psychological review.

[123]  S. Andrews Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Activation or search? , 1989 .

[124]  Robin K. Morris,et al.  Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[125]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  The broth in my brother’s brothel: Morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[126]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy? , 1987 .

[127]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Letter-position coding in random consonant arrays , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[128]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Transposed-letter effects: Consonants, vowels and letter frequency , 2008 .

[129]  J. Bowers,et al.  Automatic semantic activation of embedded words: Is there a hat in that? , 2005 .

[130]  J. Grainger,et al.  Sequential Effects of Phonological Priming in Visual Word Recognition , 2005, Psychological science.

[131]  Brenda Rapp,et al.  Consonants and vowels in orthographic representations , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[132]  T. M. Ellison,et al.  Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. , 2000, Psychological review.

[133]  Manuel Carreiras,et al.  Do transposed-letter similarity effects occur at a syllable level? , 2006, Experimental psychology.

[134]  Marcus Taft,et al.  Lexical access-via an orthographic code: The basic orthographic syllabic structure (BOSS) , 1979 .

[135]  K. Rastle,et al.  Masked phonological priming effects in English: Are they real? Do they matter? , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[136]  D. Hooper,et al.  The use of assembled phonology during performance of a letter recognition task and its dependence on the presence and proportion of word stimuli , 1997 .

[137]  Chris Davis,et al.  The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: interference effects from a masked prime , 1991 .

[138]  Clive Frankish,et al.  SIHGT and SUNOD: The role of orthography and phonology in the perception of transposed letter anagrams , 2007 .

[139]  M. Sigman,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .

[140]  J. Grainger Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming. , 1990 .

[141]  J. Bowers Masked priming: State of the Art , 2003 .

[142]  Colin J. Davis,et al.  Factors underlying masked priming effects in competitive network models of visual word recognition , 2003 .

[143]  Kenneth I. Forster,et al.  Masked form priming with extreme transposition , 2008 .

[144]  A. Jacobs,et al.  Testing a semistochastic variant of the interactive activation model in different word recognition experiments. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[145]  J. Grainger,et al.  Phonology and Orthography in Visual Word Recognition: Effects of Masked Homophone Primes , 1994 .

[146]  Max Coltheart,et al.  Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .

[147]  Jason R. Perry,et al.  An evaluation of the interactive-activation model using masked partial-word priming , 2008 .

[148]  S. Lupker,et al.  Masked inhibitory priming in english: evidence for lexical inhibition. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[149]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Temporal Integration of Information in Orthographic Priming , 1999 .

[150]  James J. Clark,et al.  Word ambiguity and the optimal viewing position in reading , 1999, Vision Research.

[151]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Order Relations in Perceptual Matching , 2005 .

[152]  K R Paap,et al.  Word shape's in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[153]  J. R. Doyle Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening , parafoveal vision , and visual masking : A survey and appraisal , 2008 .