Adaptation in the processing of interaural time differences revealed by the auditory localization aftereffect.

Two experiments were conducted involving the auditory localization aftereffect, in which the perceptual lateralization of a test sound having an interaural time difference (ITD) shifts away from that of a prior adapting sound having a different ITD. First, the frequency selectivity of the aftereffect was examined for sinusoids presented through headphones, with various combinations of adapter and test frequencies below 800 Hz, using the method of constant stimuli. The magnitude of the aftereffect was found to be largest when the frequencies of the two tones were similar, and virtually disappeared at a frequency difference of one-half octave. Second, the ITD selectivity of the aftereffect was examined for 400-Hz sinusoids. Subjects' judgments of lateralization were measured directly in terms of the perceived azimuth of the test tone for various combinations of adapter and test ITDs in the range of +/- 625 microseconds. The magnitude of the aftereffect was found to be largest when adapter and test ITDs differed by approximately 250 microseconds. These results were successfully simulated by an interaural cross-correlation model having gain control. The results are consistent with the idea that the gain of ITD-selective units, located after binaural interaction but before across-frequency integration, is changed by recent input.

[1]  L A JEFFRESS,et al.  A place theory of sound localization. , 1948, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[2]  W. Lindemann Extension of a binaural cross-correlation model by contralateral inhibition. I. Simulation of lateralization for stationary signals. , 1986, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[3]  N. Sutherland Figural After-Effects and Apparent Size , 1961 .

[4]  F S Werblin,et al.  The control of sensitivity in the retina. , 1973, Scientific American.

[5]  M. Coltheart Visual feature-analyzers and after-effects of tilt and curvature. , 1971, Psychological review.

[6]  M. Konishi,et al.  A circuit for detection of interaural time differences in the brain stem of the barn owl , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  B W Tansley,et al.  Time course of adaptation and recovery of channels selectively sensitive to frequency and amplitude modulation. , 1983, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[8]  Ray Meddis,et al.  Across frequency integration in a model of lateralization , 1992 .

[9]  W R Thurlow,et al.  Some determinants of localization-adaptation effects for successive auditory stimuli. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  D Regan,et al.  Selective adaptation to frequency-modulated tones: evidence for an information-processing channel selectively sensitive to frequency changes. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  N. V. Swindale,et al.  Spectral motion produces an auditory after-effect , 1993, Nature.

[12]  H S Colburn,et al.  Theory of binaural interaction based in auditory-nerve data. IV. A model for subjective lateral position. , 1978, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  R. Kay,et al.  On the existence in human auditory pathways of channels selectively tuned to the modulation present in frequency‐modulated tones , 1972, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Lateral Inhibition between Orientation Detectors in the Human Visual System , 1970, Nature.

[15]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Size Adaptation: A New Aftereffect , 1969, Science.

[16]  D. Grantham Motion aftereffects with horizontally moving sound sources in the free field , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.