The role of verbal behavior in human learning: infant performance on fixed-interval schedules.

The performances of two infants less than one year old were investigated on fixed-interval schedules. When the infants touched a cylinder either music or food was presented according to fixed-interval schedules ranging in value from 10 to 50 seconds. With respect to two principal criteria, namely, pattern of responding and sensitivity to the schedule parameter, the subjects' behavior closely resembled that of animals but differed markedly from that of older children and adults. Negatively accelerated responding in the course of the fixed interval in the early sessions gave way to a scalloped pattern, consisting of a pause after reinforcement followed by an accelerated response rate. This scalloped pattern was the final form of responding on all schedule values. Analysis of data after performance had stabilized showed that postreinforcement pause was a negatively accelerated increasing function, and running rate (calculated after excluding the postreinforcement pause) was a declining function, of schedule value. On each schedule, the durations of mean successive interresponse times declined in the course of the fixed interval and were directly related to schedule value. The results supported Lowe's (1979) suggestion that verbal behavior may be responsible for major differences in the schedule performance of older humans and animals.

[1]  R. M. Elliott,et al.  Behavior of Organisms , 1991 .

[2]  R Poppen,et al.  Human fixed-interval performance with concurrently programmed schedules: A parametric analysis. , 1982, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[3]  R. Poppen,et al.  Human performance on conjunctive fixed-interval fixed-ratio schedules. , 1982, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[4]  B. A. Matthews,et al.  Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity of low-rate performance to schedule contingencies. , 1981, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[5]  C. Lowe,et al.  Temporal control of behavior and the power law. , 1979, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[6]  C. Lowe,et al.  Determinants of Operant Behaviour in Humans: Some Differences from Animals , 1978, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  C F Lowe,et al.  Species differences in temporal control of behavior II: human performance. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[8]  C. Lowe,et al.  Species differences in temporal control of behavior. , 1977, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[9]  D. Meichenbaum Cognitive-Behavior Modification: An Integrative Approach , 1977 .

[10]  B. A. Matthews,et al.  Uninstructed human responding: sensitivity to ratio and interval contingencies. , 1977, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[11]  H. Terrace,et al.  WHAT MIGHT BE LEARNED FROM STUDYING LANGUAGE IN THE CHIMPANZEE? THE IMPORTANCE OF SYMBOLIZING ONESELF , 1976 .

[12]  A. Decasper,et al.  Steady-State Behavior in Children: A Method and Some Data. , 1972 .

[13]  H. Weiner Human Behavioral Persistence , 1970 .

[14]  Michael D. Zeiler,et al.  Fixed-ratio and fixed-interval schedules of cartoon presentation , 1969 .

[15]  H Weiner,et al.  Controlling human fixed-interval performance. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[16]  L. Lippman,et al.  Fixed Interval Performance as Related to Subjects’ Verbalizations of the Reinforcement Contingency , 1968 .

[17]  L. Lippman,et al.  Fixed interval performance as related to instructions and to subjects’ verbalizations of the contingency , 1967 .

[18]  H. Weiner Conditioning History and Maladaptive Human Operant Behavior , 1965, Psychological reports.

[19]  H WEINER,et al.  CONDITIONING HISTORY AND HUMAN FIXED-INTERVAL PERFORMANCE. , 1964, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[20]  S. Luria,et al.  Biosynthesis of beta-D-galactosidase controlled by phage-carried genes. III. Dereprssion of beta-d-galactosidase synthesis following induction of phage development in lysogenic bacteria. , 1961, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  R F HEFFERLINE,et al.  Escape and Avoidance Conditioning in Human Subjects without Their Observation of the Response , 1959, Science.

[22]  B J Campbell,et al.  Intermittent reinforcement of operant behavior in children. , 1958, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[23]  J. Holland,et al.  Human vigilance: the rate of observing an instrument is controlled by the schedule of signal detections. , 1958, Science.

[24]  G. Mead,et al.  Mind, Self and Society. From the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist. , 1935 .

[25]  S. Hanson,et al.  Measurement and modeling of behavior under fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement. , 1981 .

[26]  P B Dews,et al.  Studies on responding under fixed-interval schedules of reinforcement: II. The scalloped pattern of the cumulative record. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[27]  D. Meichenbaum Cognitive-Behavior Modification , 1977 .

[28]  W. Brewer There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical conditioning in adult humans. , 1974 .

[29]  A. N. Sokolov,et al.  Inner Speech and Thought , 1972 .

[30]  B. Skinner Contingencies of reinforcement : a theoretical analysis , 1969 .

[31]  H. Weiner RESPONSE COST AND FIXED-RATIO PERFORMANCE. , 1964, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[32]  R. J. Terrell,et al.  The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior , 1960 .

[33]  C. B. Ferster,et al.  Schedules of reinforcement , 1957 .