Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] G. S. Klein,et al. SEMANTIC POWER MEASURED THROUGH THE INTERFERENCE OF WORDS WITH COLOR-NAMING. , 1964, The American journal of psychology.
[2] S. Tipper. The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[3] Matthew Flatt,et al. PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .
[4] A. Caramazza. How many levels of processing are there in lexical access , 1997 .
[5] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Cascade processes in picture identification , 1988 .
[6] E. Kaplan,et al. The “Tip-of-the-Tongue” Phenomenon in Aphasia , 1976, Cortex.
[7] Gary S. Dell,et al. Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data , 1981 .
[8] J. Jescheniak,et al. Discrete serial versus cascaded processing in lexical access in speech production : Further evidence from the coactivation of near-synonyms , 1998 .
[9] Peter W. Jusczyk. The development of word recognition , 1994, ICSLP.
[10] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[11] M. Garrett. Levels of processing in sentence production , 1980 .
[12] Willem J. M. Levelt,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[13] G S Dell,et al. A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.
[14] Antje S. Meyer,et al. The time course of lexical access in speech production: A study of picture naming , 1991 .
[15] S. Lupker. The role of phonetic and orthographic similarity in picture-word interference. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.
[16] A. Roelofs,et al. A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.
[17] Victoria A. Fromkin,et al. The Non-Anomalous Nature of Anomalous Utterances , 1971 .
[18] M. Miozzo,et al. The two-stage model of lexical retrieval: evidence from a case of anomia with selective preservation of grammatical gender , 1995, Cognition.
[19] W. Levelt,et al. Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: Comment on Starreveld and La Heij (1995) , 1996 .
[20] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Interaction between semantic and orthographic factors in conceptually driven naming: Comment on Starreveld and La Heij (1995) , 1996 .
[21] Joseph Paul Stemberger,et al. Bound morpheme loss errors in normal and agrammatic speech: One mechanism or two? , 1985, Brain and Language.
[22] P. Briggs,et al. The development of word recognition processes. , 1984, British journal of psychology.
[23] J. Stroop. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .
[24] B. Rapp,et al. Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. , 2000, Psychological review.
[25] A. Caramazza,et al. The cognate facilitation effect: implications for models of lexical access. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[26] V. Ferreira,et al. Semantic and phonological information flow in the production lexicon. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[27] A. Ellis,et al. A cognitive neuropsychological case study of anomia. Implications for psychological models of word retrieval. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[28] W. Glaser,et al. Context effects in stroop-like word and picture processing. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[29] Wido La Heij,et al. Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation, and their interaction in naming tasks. , 1995 .
[30] Trevor A. Harley,et al. Phonological activation of semantic competitors during lexical access in speech production , 1993 .
[31] Julio Santiago,et al. Constraining production theories: Principled motivation, consistency, homunculi, underspecification, failed predictions, and contrary data , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[32] Brian Butterworth,et al. Disorders of phonological encoding , 1992, Cognition.
[33] G. Dell,et al. The sequential cuing effect in speech production , 1994, Cognition.
[34] Brian Butterworth,et al. Lexical access in speech production , 1989 .
[35] Gerard Kempen,et al. The lexicalization process in sentence production and naming: indirect election of words , 1983, Cognition.
[36] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[37] Antje S. Meyer,et al. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production : Picture word interference studies , 1990 .
[38] K Rayner,et al. Stages of processing in word identification. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[39] G. Dell,et al. Origins of Paraphasias in Deep Dysphasia: Testing the Consequences of a Decay Impairment to an Interactive Spreading Activation Model of Lexical Retrieval , 1994, Brain and Language.
[40] F N Dyer,et al. The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the stlldy of perceptual, cognitive, and response processes , 1973, Memory & cognition.
[41] William D. Marslen-Wilson,et al. Lexical Representation and Process , 1991 .
[42] Albert Costa,et al. Set size and repetition in the picture–word interference paradigm: implications for models of naming , 2001, Cognition.
[43] R. R. Peterson,et al. Lexical selection and phonological encoding during language production: Evidence for cascaded processing. , 1998 .