Differences in illumination estimation in #thedress.

We investigated whether people who report different colors for #thedress do so because they have different assumptions about the illumination in #thedress scene. We introduced a spherical illumination probe (Koenderink, Pont, van Doorn, Kappers, & Todd, 2007) into the original photograph, placed in fore-, or background of the scene and-for each location-let observers manipulate the probe's chromaticity, intensity and the direction of the illumination. Their task was to adjust the probe such that it would appear as a white sphere in the scene. When the probe was located in the foreground, observers who reported the dress to be white (white perceivers) tended to produce bluer adjustments than observers who reported it as blue (blue perceivers). Blue perceivers tended to perceive the illumination as less chromatic. There were no differences in chromaticity settings between perceiver types for the probe placed in the background. Perceiver types also did not differ in their illumination intensity and direction estimates across probe locations. These results provide direct support for the idea that the ambiguity in the perceived color of the dress can be explained by the different assumptions that people have about the illumination chromaticity in the foreground of the scene. In a second experiment we explore the possibility that blue perceivers might overall be less sensitive to contextual cues, and measure white and blue perceivers' dress color matches and labels for manipulated versions of the original photo. Results indeed confirm that contextual cues predominantly affect white perceivers.

[1]  Katja Doerschner,et al.  Surface color perception in three-dimensional scenes , 2006, Visual Neuroscience.

[2]  Huib de Ridder,et al.  The visual light field in paintings of Museum Prinsenhof: comparing settings in empty space and on objects , 2015, Electronic Imaging.

[3]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  Local and global processes in surface lightness perception , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Graham D. Finlayson,et al.  Chromatic Illumination Discrimination Ability Reveals that Human Colour Constancy Is Optimised for Blue Daylight Illuminations , 2014, PloS one.

[5]  D. Foster Color constancy , 2011, Vision Research.

[6]  G. Kanji,et al.  The development of analysis of variance for circular data , 1988 .

[7]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[8]  David H Brainard,et al.  Surface color perception and equivalent illumination models. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[9]  Sylvia C Pont,et al.  The Visual Light Field , 2007, Perception.

[10]  Katja Doerschner,et al.  Cues to an equivalent lighting model. , 2006, Journal of vision.

[11]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Striking individual differences in color perception uncovered by ‘the dress’ photograph , 2015, Current Biology.

[12]  David H Brainard,et al.  Perception of color and material properties in complex scenes. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[13]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[14]  Katja Doerschner,et al.  Human observers compensate for secondary illumination originating in nearby chromatic surfaces. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[15]  Christoph Witzel,et al.  The most reasonable explanation of "the dress": Implicit assumptions about illumination. , 2017, Journal of vision.

[16]  Dragan Sekulovski,et al.  The global structure of the visual light field and its relation to the physical light field. , 2016, Journal of vision.

[17]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  An anchoring theory of lightness perception. , 1999, Psychological review.

[18]  Matteo Toscani,et al.  The many colours of ‘the dress’ , 2015, Current Biology.

[19]  David H. Brainard,et al.  Colour Vision: Understanding #TheDress , 2015, Current Biology.

[20]  Michael A. Webster,et al.  Supplemental Information : Asymmetries in blue-yellow color perception and in the color of “ the dress ” , 2015 .

[21]  Matteo Valsecchi,et al.  Variations in daylight as a contextual cue for estimating season, time of day, and weather conditions. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[22]  Huseyin Boyaci,et al.  The effect of an illuminant direction cue based on cast shadows on lightness perception in three-dimensional scenes , 2004 .

[23]  D H Brainard,et al.  Color constancy in the nearly natural image. I. Asymmetric matches. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[24]  L. Maloney Physics-based approaches to modeling surface color perception , 1999 .

[25]  D. Macleod,et al.  What is white? , 2015, Journal of vision.

[26]  Shing-Chow Chan,et al.  Light Field , 2014, Computer Vision, A Reference Guide.

[27]  H. Boyaci,et al.  Testing limits on matte surface color perception in three-dimensional scenes with complex light fields , 2007, Vision Research.

[28]  A. Chetverikov,et al.  Seeing “the Dress” in the Right Light: Perceived Colors and Inferred Light Sources , 2016, Perception.

[29]  D. Macleod,et al.  Ideal white can be yellowish or bluish, but not reddish or greenish , 2010 .

[30]  A. Hurlbert Colour vision: Is colour constancy real? , 1999, Current Biology.

[31]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Object knowledge modulates colour appearance , 2011, i-Perception.

[32]  P. Lennie,et al.  Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  Ling Xia,et al.  The visual light field in real scenes , 2014, i-Perception.

[34]  Marc Ebner,et al.  Color Constancy , 2007, Computer Vision, A Reference Guide.

[35]  L. Maloney,et al.  The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes. , 2003, Journal of vision.