Spatial Learning and Memory in Birds

Behavioral ecologists, well versed in addressing functional aspects of behavior, are acknowledging more and more the attention they need also to pay to mechanistic processes. One of these is the role of cognition. Song learning and imprinting are familiar examples of behaviors for which cognition plays an important role, but attention is now turning to other behaviors and a wider diversity of species. We focus here on work that investigates the nature of spatial learning and memory in the context of behaviors such as foraging and food storing. We also briefly explore the difficulties of studying cognition in the field. The common thread to all of this work is the value of using psychological techniques as tools for assessing learning and memory abilities in order to address questions of interest to behavioral ecologists.

[1]  John D. Reynolds,et al.  Female preference for preferred males is reversed under low oxygen conditions in the common goby (Pomatoschistus microps) , 1999 .

[2]  V P Bingman,et al.  Hippocampal lesions impair navigational learning in experienced homing pigeons. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  P. Slater,et al.  Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations , 1995 .

[4]  S. Healy,et al.  Memory for flowers in rufous hummingbirds: location or local visual cues? , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[5]  A. Brodin Radio-ptilochronology―tracing radioactively labelled food in feathers , 1993 .

[6]  S. West,et al.  Learning in the nectar foraging behaviour of Helicoverpa armigera , 1998 .

[7]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[8]  S. Healy,et al.  Delayed-Matching-to-Sample by Marsh Tits and Great Tits , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[9]  N. Metcalfe,et al.  Impaired flight ability—a cost of reproduction in female blue tits , 2002 .

[10]  S. Healy,et al.  Development of hippocampal specialisation in two species of tit (Parus spp.) , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  N. Clayton Hippocampal growth and maintenance depend on food-caching experience in juvenile mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli). , 2001, Behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  M. Kavaliers,et al.  Performance (re-acquisition) of a water-maze task by adult meadow voles: Effects of age of initial task acquisition and in utero environment (litter sex-ratio) , 1994, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  J. P. Webster,et al.  Fatal attraction in rats infected with Toxoplasma gondii , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  S. West,et al.  Host selection in phytophagous insects: a new explanation for learning in adults , 2001 .

[15]  D. Sherry,et al.  Spatial memory and adaptive specialization of the hippocampus , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[16]  R. Roof,et al.  Testosterone improves maze performance and induces development of a male hippocampus in females , 1992, Brain Research.

[17]  S. D. H E A L Y,et al.  Development of hippocampal specialisation in a food-storing bird , 2022 .

[18]  G. Patricelli,et al.  Dynamic mate-searching tactic allows female satin bowerbirds Ptilonorhynchus violaceus to reduce searching , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  Elizabeth A. Sandlin,et al.  Cue use affects resource subdivision among three coexisting hummingbird species , 2000 .

[20]  M. Hixon,et al.  Territory Area, Flower Density, and Time Budgeting in Hummingbirds: An Experimental and Theoretical Analysis , 1983, The American Naturalist.

[21]  C. L. Gass,et al.  Response of a traplining hummingbird to changes in nectar availability , 1999 .

[22]  J. K. Krebs,et al.  Spatial Memory of four Species of Parus: Performance in an Open-Field Analogue of a Radial Maze , 1990 .

[23]  S. Healy,et al.  Food storing and the hippocampus in Paridae. , 1996, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[24]  T. Laverty,et al.  Effect of floral symmetry on flower choice and foraging behaviour of bumble bees , 1998 .

[25]  Scott A. MacDougall-Shackleton,et al.  Comparative studies of sex differences in the song-control system of songbirds , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.

[26]  V. Braithwaite,et al.  Population differences in spatial learning in three–spined sticklebacks , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  W H Meck,et al.  Organizational effects of early gonadal secretions on sexual differentiation in spatial memory. , 1990, Behavioral neuroscience.

[28]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Organizational effects of early gonadal secretions on sexual differentiation in spatial memory. , 1990, Behavioral Neuroscience.

[29]  A C Kamil,et al.  Differences in hippocampal volume among food storing corvids. , 1996, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[30]  J. Bolhuis,et al.  The evolution of intelligence: adaptive specializations versus general process , 2001, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[31]  C. L. Gass,et al.  Specialization by territorial hummingbirds on experimentally enriched patches of flowers: energetic profitability and learning , 1985 .

[32]  R. Menzel,et al.  Symmetry perception in an insect , 1996, Nature.

[33]  M. Kavaliers,et al.  Gonadal Hormone Levels and Spatial Learning Performance in the Morris Water Maze in Male and Female Meadow Voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus , 1995, Hormones and Behavior.

[34]  S. Healy,et al.  Hippocampal specialization of food-storing birds. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  V. Braithwaite,et al.  The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[36]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Imprinting, learning and development: from behaviour to brain and back , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[37]  E. Macphail,et al.  The Avian Hippocampus and Short-Term Memory for Spatial and Non-Spatial Information , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[38]  N. Franks,et al.  Food Hoarding in Animals , 1990 .

[39]  Thomas J. Valone,et al.  Information for patch assessment: a field investigation with black-chinned hummingbirds , 1992 .

[40]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Risk-sensitivity: crossroads for theories of decision-making , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  D. Sherry,et al.  Long-term memory for cache sites in the black-capped chickadee , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[42]  F. Gill,et al.  TRAPLINE FORAGING BY HERMIT HUMMINGBIRDS: COMPETITION FOR AN UNDEFENDED, RENEWABLE RESOURCE' , 1988 .

[43]  David F. Sherry,et al.  Hippocampus and memory for food caches in black-capped chickadees , 1989 .

[44]  S. Healy Memory for Objects and Positions: Delayed Non-matching-to-sample in Storing and Non-storing Tits , 1995 .

[45]  S. West,et al.  Learning in Helicoverpa armigera (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae): a new look at the behaviour and control of a polyphagous pest , 1999 .

[46]  G. Ruxton,et al.  Testing Müllerian mimicry: an experiment with wild birds , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[47]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays , 1998, Nature.

[48]  John R. Krebs,et al.  Food storing by marsh tits , 1981, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  R. Godard Long-term memory of individual neighbours in a migratory songbird , 1991, Nature.

[50]  K. Norris,et al.  Ecological immunology: life history trade-offs and immune defense in birds , 2000 .

[51]  L. L. Wolf,et al.  Non-Random Foraging by Hummingbirds: Patterns of Movement Between Ipomopsis aggregata (Pursch) V. Grant Inflorescences , 1990 .

[52]  E. A. Sandlin Foraging information affects the nature of competitive interactions , 2000 .

[53]  D. Wilkie,et al.  Characterization of the strategy used by rats in an interval time-place learning task. , 1998 .

[54]  S. Healy,et al.  A larger hippocampus is associated with longer-lasting spatial memory , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[55]  T. Slagsvold,et al.  Sexual Imprinting and the Origin of Obligate Brood Parasitism in Birds , 2001, The American Naturalist.

[56]  D. Kimura,et al.  The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive ability patterns , 1991, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[57]  N. Clayton Development of memory and the hippocampus: comparison of food-storing and nonstoring birds on a one-trial associative memory task , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[58]  C. Rowe Sound improves visual discrimination learning in avian predators , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[59]  S. Healy,et al.  Hippocampal volume in migratory and non-migratory warblers: effects of age and experience , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[60]  J. Krebs,et al.  Memory for spatial and object-specific cues in food-storing and non-storing birds , 1994, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[61]  S. Healy,et al.  ‘Neuroecologists’ are not made of straw , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[62]  C. Barnes Aging and the physiology of spatial memory , 1988, Neurobiology of Aging.

[63]  C. ten Cate,et al.  Sexual imprinting and evolutionary processes in birds: A reassessment , 1999 .

[64]  N. Clayton,et al.  A reply to the defenders of the faith , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  D. Wilkie,et al.  Rats use an ordinal timer in a daily time-place learning task. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[66]  S. Shettleworth,et al.  Hippocampus and memory in a food-storing and in a nonstoring bird species. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[67]  M. Eens,et al.  Effects of Testosterone on Song, Aggression, and Nestling Feeding Behavior in Male Great Tits, Parus major , 2002, Hormones and Behavior.

[68]  G. Ball,et al.  Having to revise hypotheses does not indicate a flawed approach: a reply to Bolhuis and Macphail , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[69]  D. Sherry,et al.  The hippocampal complex of food-storing birds. , 1989, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[70]  R. J. Barfield Gonadotrophic hormone secretion in the female ring dove in response to visual and auditory stimulation by the male. , 1971, The Journal of endocrinology.

[71]  S. Healy,et al.  Food storing and the hippocampus in corvids: amount and volume are correlated , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[72]  Daniel T. Blumstein,et al.  Training Captive‐Bred or Translocated Animals to Avoid Predators , 2000 .

[73]  S. Healy,et al.  Comparing spatial memory in two species of tit: Recalling a single positive location , 1992 .

[74]  Jonathan Henderson,et al.  Rufous hummingbirds' memory for flower location , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[75]  Lucia F. Jacobs,et al.  Visual environment and delay affect cache retrieval accuracy in a food-storing rodent , 1998 .

[76]  J. Juraska,et al.  Differential rearing experience, gender, and radial maze performance. , 1984, Developmental psychobiology.

[77]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Risky Theories—The Effects of Variance on Foraging Decisions , 1996 .

[78]  R. Roof,et al.  Neonatal exogenous testosterone modifies sex difference in radial arm and morris water maze performance in prepubescent and adult rats , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[79]  J. Crystal,et al.  Simultaneous temporal and spatial processing , 2002, Animal learning & behavior.

[80]  J. Ekman,et al.  Benefits of food hoarding , 1994, Nature.

[81]  S. Healy,et al.  Spatial accuracy in food-storing and nonstoring birds , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[82]  S. Schantz,et al.  Cognitive effects of endocrine-disrupting chemicals in animals. , 2001, Environmental health perspectives.

[83]  T. Andrew Hurly,et al.  Context-dependent, risk-sensitive foraging preferences in wild rufous hummingbirds , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[84]  D S Olton,et al.  Spatial memory. , 1977, Scientific American.

[85]  David F. Sherry,et al.  Food storage by birds and mammals , 1985 .

[86]  S. Shettleworth,et al.  Hippocampal lesions impair memory for location but not color in passerine birds. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[87]  Michael J. Ryan,et al.  Male body length influences mate-choice copying in the sailfin molly Poecilia latipinna , 1998 .

[88]  RUFOUS HUMMINGBIRDS' (SELASPHORUS RUFUS) MEMORY FOR FLOWERS : PATTERNS OR ACTUAL SPATIAL LOCATIONS? , 1998 .

[89]  D. Sherry,et al.  Females have a larger hippocampus than males in the brood-parasitic brown-headed cowbird. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[90]  Jonathan I. Flombaum,et al.  Neuroecology and psychological modularity , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[91]  D. Brodbeck Memory for spatial and local cues: A comparison of a storing and a nonstoring species , 1994 .

[92]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Species and sex differences in hippocampus size in parasitic and non‐parasitic cowbirds , 1996, Neuroreport.

[93]  G. Patricelli,et al.  Complex Mate Searching in the Satin Bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus violaceus , 2001, The American Naturalist.

[94]  M. Mendl,et al.  Pigs in space: spatial memory and its susceptibility to interference , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[95]  T. Guilford,et al.  Navigational ability in the domestic fowl (Gallus gallus domesticus) , 2003 .

[96]  Revising hypotheses does not indicate a flawed approach. Reply to Bolhuis and Macphail. , 2002, Trends in cognitive sciences.

[97]  S. Volman,et al.  Relative hippocampal volume in relation to food-storing behavior in four species of woodpeckers. , 1997, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.

[98]  ANDREW T. HURLY,et al.  Spatial memory in rufous hummingbirds: memory for rewarded and non-rewarded sites , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[99]  S. Shettleworth,et al.  Hippocampal volume and food-storing behavior are related in parids. , 1995, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[100]  W. Meck,et al.  The organizational effects of gonadal steroids on sexually dimorphic spatial ability , 1991, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[101]  E. Curio,et al.  SEXUAL IMPRINTING SUPPORTS THE EVOLUTION OF NOVEL MALE TRAITS BY TRANSFERENCE OF A PREFERENCE FOR THE COLOUR RED , 2000 .

[102]  Johan J. Bolhuis,et al.  A critique of the neuroecology of learning and memory , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[103]  T. Roper,et al.  Effects of novel colour and smell on the response of naive chicks towards food and water , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[104]  Sara J. Shettleworth,et al.  Matching location and color of a compound stimulus: Comparison of a food-storing and a nonstoring bird species. , 1995 .

[105]  M. Weiss Cognitive Ecology of Pollination: Vision and learning in some neglected pollinators: beetles, flies, moths, and butterflies , 2001 .

[106]  T. A. Hurly,et al.  Cue learning by rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus). , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[107]  G. Rosenqvist,et al.  Prior exposure to male phenotypes influences mate choice in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata , 1997 .

[108]  T. A. Hurly,et al.  Spatial memory in rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus): A field test , 1995 .

[109]  D. Sherry Food storage by black-capped chickadees: Memory for the location and contents of caches , 1984, Animal Behaviour.

[110]  J. Crystal,et al.  Representation of time in time-place learning , 2002, Animal learning & behavior.

[111]  B. P. Moore,et al.  The biological roles of pyrazines: evidence for a warning odour function , 1987 .

[112]  J. Krebs,et al.  Hippocampal growth and attrition in birds affected by experience. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[113]  S. Yoerg Ecological Frames of Mind: The Role of Cognition in Behavioral Ecology , 1991, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[114]  C. L. Gass,et al.  Cues for orientation in hummingbird foraging: color and position , 1985 .

[115]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[116]  Glenn D. Sutherland,et al.  Learning and remembering of spatial patterns by hummingbirds , 1995, Animal Behaviour.