Cortical Thickness in Fusiform Face Area Predicts Face and Object Recognition Performance

The fusiform face area (FFA) is defined by its selectivity for faces. Several studies have shown that the response of FFA to nonface objects can predict behavioral performance for these objects. However, one possible account is that experts pay more attention to objects in their domain of expertise, driving signals up. Here, we show an effect of expertise with nonface objects in FFA that cannot be explained by differential attention to objects of expertise. We explore the relationship between cortical thickness of FFA and face and object recognition using the Cambridge Face Memory Test and Vanderbilt Expertise Test, respectively. We measured cortical thickness in functionally defined regions in a group of men who evidenced functional expertise effects for cars in FFA. Performance with faces and objects together accounted for approximately 40% of the variance in cortical thickness of several FFA patches. Whereas participants with a thicker FFA cortex performed better with vehicles, those with a thinner FFA cortex performed better with faces and living objects. The results point to a domain-general role of FFA in object perception and reveal an interesting double dissociation that does not contrast faces and objects but rather living and nonliving objects.

[1]  S. Blumstein,et al.  The Role of Segmentation in Phonological Processing: An fMRI Investigation , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  P. Rakić,et al.  Synaptogenesis in visual cortex of normal and preterm monkeys: evidence for intrinsic regulation of synaptic overproduction. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Measuring structural–functional correspondence: Spatial variability of specialised brain regions after macro-anatomical alignment , 2012, NeuroImage.

[4]  Y. Grodzinsky,et al.  The battle for Broca’s region , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  A visual short-term memory advantage for objects of expertise. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  Johan Wagemans,et al.  Activation of Fusiform Face Area by Greebles Is Related to Face Similarity but Not Expertise , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Jennifer M. D. Yoon,et al.  Functionally Defined White Matter Reveals Segregated Pathways in Human Ventral Temporal Cortex Associated with Category-Specific Processing , 2015, Neuron.

[8]  Erminio Capitani,et al.  Gender Affects Word Retrieval of Certain Categories in Semantic Fluency Tasks , 1999, Cortex.

[9]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Analysis of functional image analysis contest (FIAC) data with brainvoyager QX: From single‐subject to cortically aligned group general linear model analysis and self‐organizing group independent component analysis , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[10]  Rankin W. McGugin,et al.  High-resolution imaging of expertise reveals reliable object selectivity in the fusiform face area related to perceptual performance , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Patrick C M Wong,et al.  Volume of left Heschl's Gyrus and linguistic pitch learning. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  AC Evans,et al.  Positive association between cognitive ability and cortical thickness in a representative US sample of healthy 6 to 18 year-olds , 2009, NeuroImage.

[13]  E. Maguire,et al.  London taxi drivers and bus drivers: A structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis , 2006, Hippocampus.

[14]  T. Paus Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Francis M Miezin,et al.  Comparison of sustained and transient activity in children and adults using a mixed blocked/event-related fMRI design , 2004, NeuroImage.

[16]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  Isabelle Peretz,et al.  Morphometry of the amusic brain: a two-site study. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  Rankin W. McGugin,et al.  The Vanderbilt Expertise Test reveals domain-general and domain-specific sex effects in object recognition , 2012, Vision Research.

[19]  Cindy M. Bukach,et al.  Limits of generalization between categories and implications for theories of category specificity , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[20]  Chantal Delon-Martin,et al.  Perfumers' expertise induces structural reorganization in olfactory brain regions , 2013, NeuroImage.

[21]  Noah A. Shamosh,et al.  Multiple Bases of Human Intelligence Revealed by Cortical Thickness and Neural Activation , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[23]  Arthur W Toga,et al.  Relationships between IQ and regional cortical gray matter thickness in healthy adults. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  N. Kanwisher Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  M. Tarr,et al.  Visual expertise with nonface objects leads to competition with the early perceptual processing of faces in the human occipitotemporal cortex. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  fMRI-adaptation and category selectivity in human ventral temporal cortex: regional differences across time scales. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  M. Tarr,et al.  FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  Keith R. Laws,et al.  Gender Affects Naming Latencies for Living and Nonliving Things: Implications for Familiarity , 1999, Cortex.

[29]  D. Samuel Schwarzkopf,et al.  The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Sarah Shomstein,et al.  Parietal Cortex Mediates Voluntary Control of Spatial and Nonspatial Auditory Attention , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Neuroanatomical correlates of visual car expertise , 2012, NeuroImage.

[32]  C. Gross,et al.  Neural representations of faces and body parts in macaque and human cortex: a comparative FMRI study. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  E. Capitani,et al.  Semantic category dissociations in naming : is there a gender effect in Alzheimer’s disease? , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neuroimaging Studies of Attention: From Modulation of Sensory Processing to Top-Down Control , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  R. Haier,et al.  Neuroanatomy of creativity , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[36]  Joseph T Devlin,et al.  The myth of the visual word form area , 2003, NeuroImage.

[37]  T. Bourgeron,et al.  Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Visual Word Form and Fusiform Face Areas. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Cortical Thickness in Congenital Amusia: When Less Is Better Than More , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Rafael Malach,et al.  Top-down engagement modulates the neural expressions of visual expertise , 2010 .

[40]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Voxel-based morphometry reveals reduced grey matter volume in the temporal cortex of developmental prosopagnosics , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  Robert J. Zatorre,et al.  Cortical structure predicts success in performing musical transformation judgments , 2010, NeuroImage.

[42]  Rankin W. McGugin,et al.  Robust expertise effects in right FFA , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Yaoda Xu Revisiting the role of the fusiform face area in visual expertise. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  Taiyong Bi,et al.  Function and Structure of Human Left Fusiform Cortex Are Closely Associated with Perceptual Learning of Faces , 2014, Current Biology.

[45]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  André J. W. van der Kouwe,et al.  Reliability of MRI-derived cortical and subcortical morphometric measures: Effects of pulse sequence, voxel geometry, and parallel imaging , 2009, NeuroImage.

[47]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents , 2006, Nature.

[48]  Marc G. Berman,et al.  Evaluating functional localizers: The case of the FFA , 2010, NeuroImage.

[49]  M. Tarr,et al.  Becoming a “Greeble” Expert: Exploring Mechanisms for Face Recognition , 1997, Vision Research.

[50]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing , 1995 .

[51]  K. Nakayama,et al.  The Cambridge Face Memory Test: Results for neurologically intact individuals and an investigation of its validity using inverted face stimuli and prosopagnosic participants , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  M. Scherg,et al.  Structural and functional asymmetry of lateral Heschl's gyrus reflects pitch perception preference , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Rankin W. McGugin,et al.  Expertise Effects in Face-Selective Areas are Robust to Clutter and Diverted Attention, but not to Competition. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  Katrin Amunts,et al.  The mid-fusiform sulcus: A landmark identifying both cytoarchitectonic and functional divisions of human ventral temporal cortex , 2014, NeuroImage.

[56]  W. Grodd,et al.  Many Faces of Expertise: Fusiform Face Area in Chess Experts and Novices , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[58]  Suzanne E. Welcome,et al.  Longitudinal Mapping of Cortical Thickness and Brain Growth in Normal Children , 2022 .

[59]  Magnus Lindgren,et al.  Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning , 2012, NeuroImage.

[60]  I. Gauthier,et al.  Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[61]  Robert J Zatorre,et al.  Anatomical Correlates of Learning Novel Speech Sounds , 2002, Neuron.

[62]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[63]  P. McKenna,et al.  Category specificity in the naming of natural and man-made objects: Normative data from adults and children , 1994 .

[64]  Isabel Gauthier,et al.  Individual differences in FFA activity suggest independent processing at different spatial scales , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[65]  I. Aharon,et al.  Three‐dimensional mapping of cortical thickness using Laplace's Equation , 2000, Human brain mapping.