Specific cognitive deficits in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive–compulsive disorder

Synopsis Forty patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) were compared to matched healthy controls on neuropsychological tests which are sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. On a computerized version of the Tower of London test of planning, the patients were no different from healthy controls in the accuracy of their solutions. However, when they made a mistake, they spent more time than the controls in generating alternative solutions or checking that the next move would be correct. The results suggest that OCD patients have a selective deficit in generating alternative strategies when they make a mistake. In a separate attentional set-shifting task, OCD patients were impaired in a simple discrimination learning task and showed a continuous cumulative increase in the number who failed at each stage of the task, including the crucial extra-dimensional set shifting stage. This suggests that OCD patients show deficits in both acquiring and maintaining cognitive sets. The cognitive deficits in OCD may be summarized as: (i) being easily distracted by other competing stimuli; (ii) excessive monitoring and checking of the response to ensure a mistake does not occur; and (iii) when a mistake does occur, being more rigid at setting aside the main goal and planning the necessary subgoals. Both studies support the evidence of fronto-striatal dysfunction in OCD and the results are discussed in terms of an impaired Supervisory Attentional System.

[1]  T. Robbins,et al.  Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[2]  B. Sahakian,et al.  Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[3]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[4]  P. Salkovskis,et al.  The Relationship Between Obsessional Problems and Intrusive Thoughts , 1995, Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy.

[5]  M Ashtari,et al.  Reduced caudate nucleus volume in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1995, Archives of general psychiatry.

[6]  D. Black The Psychobiology of Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder , 1993 .

[7]  D. Veale Classification and Treatment of Obsessional Slowness , 1993, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[8]  C. Marsden,et al.  Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson's disease. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  A. Lees,et al.  The neurology of obsessional slowness. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[10]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Obsessional slowness. Functional studies with positron emission tomography. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  M. Taylor,et al.  Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder , 1991 .

[12]  T. Robbins,et al.  Sparing of attentional relative to mnemonic function in a subgroup of patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  A. C. Roberts,et al.  Impaired extra-dimensional shift performance in medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a specific attentional dysfunction , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  J. Rapoport,et al.  Cerebral glucose metabolism in childhood-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[16]  D. Bolton,et al.  Deficit in cognitive shifting ability in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder , 1989, Biological Psychiatry.

[17]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Reduction of prefrontal cortex glucose metabolism common to three types of depression. , 1989, Archives of general psychiatry.

[18]  T. Nordahl,et al.  Cerebral glucose metabolic rates in obsessive compulsive disorder. , 1989, Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology.

[19]  Judith L. Rapoport,et al.  Obsessive-compulsive disorder in children and adolescents , 1989 .

[20]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cerebral glucose metabolic rates in nondepressed patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1988, The American journal of psychiatry.

[21]  J. Rapoport,et al.  Neuroanatomical abnormalities in obsessive-compulsive disorder detected with quantitative X-ray computed tomography. , 1988, The American journal of psychiatry.

[22]  T. Robbins,et al.  Planning and spatial working memory in Parkinson's disease. , 1988, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[23]  I. Marks Behavioural psychotherapy: Maudsley pocket book of clinical management. , 1986 .

[24]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  M. Albert,et al.  Varieties of perseveration , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  F. Boller,et al.  Visuospatial impairment in Parkinson's disease. Role of perceptual and motor factors. , 1984, Archives of neurology.

[27]  J L Rapoport,et al.  Computerized tomography and neuropsychological test measures in adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1984, The American journal of psychiatry.

[28]  I. Creese Stimulants, neurochemical, behavioral, and clinical perspectives , 1983 .

[29]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  Z. Koles,et al.  Neuropsychological and power spectral EEG investigations of the obsessive-compulsive syndrome. , 1979, Biological psychiatry.

[31]  T. Robbins A Critique of the Methods Available for the Measurement of Spontaneous Motor Activity , 1977 .

[32]  N. Nie,et al.  Statistical Package for the Social Sciences , 1970 .

[33]  Seymour Geisser,et al.  Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1963 .

[34]  S. Kullback Information Theory and Statistics , 1959 .