Sequential control signals determine arm and trunk contributions to hand transport during reaching in humans

When reaching towards objects placed outside the arm workspace, the trunk assumes an active role in transport of the hand by contributing to the extent of movement while simultaneously maintaining the direction of reach. We investigated the spatial‐temporal aspects of the integration of the trunk motion into reaching. Specifically, we tested the hypothesis that the efficiency (‘gain’) of the arm‐trunk co‐ordination determining the contribution of the trunk to the extent of hand movement may vary substantially with the phase of reaching. Sitting subjects made fast pointing movements towards ipsi‐ and a contralateral targets placed beyond the reach of the right arm so that a forward trunk motion was required to assist in transporting the hand to the target. Sight of the arm and target was blocked before the movement onset. In randomly selected trials, the trunk motion was unexpectedly prevented by an electromagnet. Subjects were instructed to make stereotypical movements whether or not the trunk was arrested. In non‐perturbed trials, most subjects began to move the hand and trunk simultaneously. In trunk‐blocked trials, it was impossible for the hand to cover the whole pointing distance but the hand trajectory and velocity profile initially matched those from the trials in which the trunk motion was free, approximately until the hand reached its peak velocity. The arm inter‐joint co‐ordination substantially changed in response to the trunk arrest at a minimal latency of 40 ms after the perturbation onset. The results suggest that when the trunk was free, the influence of the trunk motion on the hand trajectory and velocity profile was initially neutralized by appropriate changes in the arm joint angles. Only after the hand had reached its peak velocity did the trunk contribute to the extent of pointing. Previous studies suggested that the central commands underlying the transport component of arm movements are completed when the hand reaches peak velocity. These studies, together with the present finding that the trunk only begins to contribute to the hand displacement at peak hand velocity, imply that the central commands that determine the contributions of the arm and the trunk to the transport of the hand are generated sequentially, even though the arm and trunk move in parallel.

[1]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Equilibrium-Point Control Hypothesis Examined by Measured Arm Stiffness During Multijoint Movement , 1996, Science.

[2]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  The influence of different descending systems on the tonic stretch reflex in the cat. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[3]  C. Chandler,et al.  Computers, brains and the control of movement , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.

[4]  S. Hagiwara,et al.  The calcium channel , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[5]  O. I. Fukson,et al.  The spinal frog takes into account the scheme of its body during the wiping reflex. , 1980, Science.

[6]  M. Arbib,et al.  Role of the cerebellum in reaching movements in humans. I. Distributed inverse dynamics control , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  The timing of control signals underlying fast point-to-point arm movements , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  G. E. Stelmach,et al.  Prehension with trunk assisted reaching , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[9]  M. Berkinblit,et al.  Pointing to remembered targets in 3-D space in Parkinson's disease. , 1998, Motor control.

[10]  George E. Stelmach,et al.  Spatial and temporal control of trunk-assisted prehensile actions , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Postural invariance in three-dimensional reaching and grasping movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  M. Levin,et al.  Deficits in the coordination of agonist and antagonist muscles in stroke patients: implications for normal motor control , 2000, Brain Research.

[13]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  The timing of arm-trunk coordination is deficient and vision-dependent in Parkinson's patients during reaching movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  M. Latash Control of human movement , 1993 .

[15]  P. Pigeon,et al.  Recruitment and sequencing of different degrees of freedom during pointing movements involving the trunk in healthy and hemiparetic subjects , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  A. Brodal,et al.  Anatomy of the Vestibular Nuclei and their Connections , 1974 .

[17]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  The origin and use of positional frames of reference in motor control , 1995, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Postural adjustments associated with different unloadings of the forearm: effects of proprioceptive and cutaneous afferent deprivation. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[19]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Coordination among the body segments during reach-to-grasp action involving the trunk , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  J. Lackner Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  F Lacquaniti,et al.  Neck muscle vibration makes walking humans accelerate in the direction of gaze , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  K. Lashley The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .

[23]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Multi-muscle control of head movements in monkeys: the referent configuration hypothesis , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[24]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  Interaction of rhythmic and discrete pattern generators in single-joint movements , 2000 .

[25]  R. Fitzpatrick,et al.  Task‐dependent reflex responses and movement illusions evoked by galvanic vestibular stimulation in standing humans. , 1994, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  J. Hollerbach Computers, brains and the control of movement , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.

[27]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Superposition of independent units of coordination during pointing movements involving the trunk with and without visual feedback , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  M. Latash,et al.  An equilibrium-point model for fast, single-joint movement: II. Similarity of single-joint isometric and isotonic descending commands. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  M. Turvey,et al.  Interlimb coupling in a simple serial behavior: A task dynamic approach , 1998 .

[30]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Control of Trajectory Modifications in Target-Directed Reaching. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  T. R. Kaminski,et al.  The coordination between trunk and arm motion during pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  C A Fowler,et al.  Coordination and Coarticulation in Speech Production , 1993, Language and speech.

[33]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[34]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Control processes underlying elbow flexion movements may be independent of kinematic and electromyographic patterns: experimental study and modelling , 1997, Neuroscience.

[35]  P. Matthews The dependence of tension upon extension in the stretch reflex of the soleus muscle of the decerebrate cat , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[36]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Recent Tests of the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) , 1998 .

[37]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[38]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  S Ma,et al.  Two functionally different synergies during arm reaching movements involving the trunk. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Howard Poizner,et al.  Hand trajectory invariance in reaching movements involving the trunk , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Identifying the control structure of multijoint coordination during pistol shooting , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  D. Guitton,et al.  Control of eye—head coordination during orienting gaze shifts , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[43]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[44]  J. Sharpe,et al.  The initial vestibulo-ocular reflex and its visual enhancement and cancellation in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  D J Ostry,et al.  Are complex control signals required for human arm movement? , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  Neville Hogan,et al.  Stability properties of human reaching movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Recent tests of the equilibrium-point hypothesis (lambda model). , 1998, Motor control.

[48]  Pascale Pigeon,et al.  Compensatory arm–trunk coordination in pointing movements is preserved in the absence of visual feedback , 1998, Brain Research.

[49]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[50]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Reaching beyond reach , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Control variables and proprioceptive feedback in fast single-joint movement. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[52]  O. I. Fukson,et al.  Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[53]  J. Kelso,et al.  Functionally specific articulatory cooperation following jaw perturbations during speech: evidence for coordinative structures. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Merging different motor patterns: Coordination between rhythmical and discrete single-joint movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.