Ultrasound-triggered, flight-gated evasive maneuvers in the praying mantis Parasphendale agrionina. I. Free flight.

Free-flying male praying mantises Parasphendale agrionina (Gerst.) perform evasive maneuvers when stimulated by ultrasound and when attacked by hunting, echolocating bats. They do not, however, respond in any way when standing on a substratum. The maneuvers are graded in intensity with distance from the sound source: far from the source they are simple turns, whereas close to the source they are steep diving turns or spirals. The maneuvers are made under power, and the male's velocity doubles to almost 4 m s-1 by the end of a steep dive. The mantis does not show any directional preference. The behavioral threshold of 64 dB SPL and minimum latency to course change of 125 ms indicate that these mantises should have adequate time to evade bats using calls of greater than 85-90 dB SPL (at 10 cm). In field experiments with wild, hunting bats, P. agrionina successfully evaded capture in all five attacks to which they responded with evasive maneuvers. Out of three attacks on P. aeruginosa and three on a normally non-responding mantis, Miomantis paykullii Stål, in which there were no evasive maneuvers, the mantis was captured in five cases.

[1]  The Response to Sound in Certain Lepidoptera , 1955 .

[2]  T. Weis-Fogh Biology and Physics of locust flight II. Flight performance of the desert locust (Schistocerca gregaria) , 1956, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences.

[3]  D. Griffin,et al.  Listening in the Dark , 1959 .

[4]  W. J. Langford Statistical Methods , 1959, Nature.

[5]  Charles R. Michael,et al.  The echolocation of flying insects by bats , 1960 .

[6]  K. D. Roeder The behaviour of free flying moths in the presence of artificial ultrasonic pulses , 1962 .

[7]  G. W. Snedecor Statistical Methods , 1964 .

[8]  Bruce J. Hayward,et al.  Flight Speeds in Western Bats , 1964 .

[9]  K. D. Roeder Aspects of the noctuid tympanic nerve response having significance in the avoidance of bats , 1964 .

[10]  K. D. Roeder Acoustic sensitivity of the noctuid tympanic organ and its range for the cries of bats. , 1966, Journal of insect physiology.

[11]  K. D. Roeder Turning tendency of moths exposed to ultrasound while in stationary flight , 1967 .

[12]  D. A. Humphries,et al.  Erratic Display as a Device against Predators , 1967, Science.

[13]  D C Sandeman,et al.  A sensitive position measuring device for biological systems. , 1968, Comparative biochemistry and physiology.

[14]  R. Greenberg Biometry , 1969, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[15]  M. Edmunds Defensive behaviour in Ghanaian praying mantids , 1972 .

[16]  J. Kingdon East African mammals : an atlas of evolution in Africa , 1972 .

[17]  GILLIAN D. SALES Acoustic orientation , 1974, Nature.

[18]  L. Miller The behaviour of flying green lacewings, Chrysopa carnea, in the presence of ultrasound , 1975 .

[19]  M. Edmunds The defensive behaviour of Ghanaian praying mantids with a discussion of territoriality , 1976 .

[20]  G. Pollack,et al.  Steering responses of flying crickets to sound and ultrasound: Mate attraction and predator avoidance. , 1978, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  M. H. Robinson,et al.  By Dawn's Early Light: Matutinal Mating and Sex Attractants in a Neotropical Mantid , 1979, Science.

[22]  C. R. Fourtner,et al.  Flight Activity Initiated via Giant Interneurons of the Cockroach: Evidence for Bifunctional Trigger Interneurons , 1980, Science.

[23]  How the Green Lacewing Avoids Bats: Behavior and Physiology , 1980 .

[24]  Nycteris grandis (Nycteridae): an African carnivorous bat , 1981 .

[25]  M. Fenton,et al.  Recognition of Species of Insectivorous Bats by Their Echolocation Calls , 1981 .

[26]  Responses of nonflying moths to ultrasound: the threat of gleaning bats , 1981 .

[27]  R. Hoy,et al.  Initiation of behavior by single neurons: the role of behavioral context. , 1984, Science.

[28]  M. Novacek Evidence for echolocation in the oldest known bats , 1985, Nature.

[29]  Kinematic and behavioural aspects of flight of the male banded agrion, Calopteryx (Agrion) splendens L , 1985 .

[30]  R. Hoy,et al.  The cyclopean ear: a new sense for the praying mantis. , 1986, Science.

[31]  The phenology and diversity of praying mantids in Ghana , 1986 .

[32]  Jacqueline J. Belwood,et al.  Foraging Behavior, Prey Selection, and Echolocation in Phyllostomine Bats (Phyllostomidae) , 1988 .

[33]  H. Spangler MOTH HEARING, DEFENSE, AND COMMUNICATION ! , 1988 .

[34]  A. Surlykke Interaction Between Echolocating Bats and Their Prey , 1988 .

[35]  H. Aldridge,et al.  Recent Advances in the Study of Bats, M. Brock Fenton, Paul Racey, Jeremy M.V. Rayner (Eds.). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1987), xii , 1988 .

[36]  H. Spangler Hearing in tiger beetles (Cicindelidae) , 1988 .

[37]  D. Robert THE AUDITORY BEHAVIOUR OF FLYING LOCUSTS , 1989 .

[38]  R. Brigham,et al.  Intraspecific Variation in the Echolocation Calls of Two Species of Insectivorous Bats , 1989 .

[39]  Roosting and Echolocation Behavior of the African Bat, Chalinolobus variegatus , 1989 .

[40]  R. Hoy,et al.  Ultrasound-induced yaw movements in the flying Australian field cricket (Teleogryllus oceanicus). , 1990, The Journal of experimental biology.

[41]  M. Brock Fenton,et al.  The foraging behaviour and ecology of animal-eating bats , 1990 .

[42]  D D Yager,et al.  Ultrasound-triggered, flight-gated evasive maneuvers in the praying mantis Parasphendale agrionina. II. Tethered flight. , 1990, The Journal of experimental biology.

[43]  D. D. Yager Sexual dimorphism of auditory function and structure in praying mantises (Mantodea; Dictyoptera) , 1990 .