Invariant cues for place of articulation in stop consonants.

In a series of experiments, identification responses for place of articulation were obtained for synthetic stop consonants in consonant-vowel syllables with different vowels. The acoustic attributes of the consonants were systematically manipulated, the selection of stimulus characteristics being guided in part by theoretical considerations concerning the expected properties of the sound generated in the vocal tract as place of articulation is varied. Several stimulus series were generated with and without noise bursts at the onset, and with and without formant transitions following consonantal release. Stimuli with transitions only, and with bursts plus transitions, were consistently classified according to place of articulation, whereas stimuli with bursts only and no transitions were not consistently identified. The acoustic attributes of the stimuli were examined to determine whether invariant properties characterized each place of atriculation independent of vowel context. It was determined that the gross shape of the spectrum sampled at the consonantal release showed a distinctive shape for each place of articulation: a prominent midfrequency spectral peak for velars, a diffuse-rising spectrum for alveolars, and a diffuse-falling spectrum for labials. These attributes are evident for stimuli containing transitions only, but are enhanced by the presence of noise bursts at the onset.

[1]  A. Liberman,et al.  Acoustic Loci and Transitional Cues for Consonants , 1954 .

[2]  Carola Schatz The Role of Context in the Perception of Stops , 1954 .

[3]  H. Hoffman Study of Some Cues in the Perception of the Voiced Stop Consonants , 1958 .

[4]  Gunnar Fant,et al.  Acoustic Theory Of Speech Production , 1960 .

[5]  A M Liberman,et al.  Perception of the speech code. , 1967, Psychological review.

[6]  A. Moffitt,et al.  Consonant cue perception by twenty- to twenty-four-week-old infants. , 1971, Child development.

[7]  P. Morse The discrimination of speech and nonspeech stimuli in early infancy. , 1972, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[8]  H Winitz,et al.  Identification of stops and vowels for the burst portion of (p,t,k) isolated from conversational speech. , 1972, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  R. Cole,et al.  Toward a theory of speech perception. , 1974, Psychological review.

[10]  Peter D. Eimas,et al.  Auditory and linguistic processing of cues for place of articulation by infants , 1974 .

[11]  Kenneth N. Stevens,et al.  Quantal Aspects of Consonant Production and Perception: A Study of Retroflex Stop Consonants. , 1975 .

[12]  K. Stevens The Potential Role of Property Detectors in the Perception of Consonants , 1975 .

[13]  B. Leshowitz,et al.  Masking patterns for continuous and gated sinusoids. , 1975, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  G. Fant,et al.  Two-formant Models, Pitch and Vowel Perception , 1975 .

[15]  Victor W. Zue,et al.  Acoustic Characteristics of Stop Consonants: A Controlled Study , 1976 .

[16]  M. Dorman,et al.  Onset spectra and stop consonant recognition , 1977 .

[17]  Kenneth N. Stevens,et al.  Acoustic invariance for place of articulation in stops and nasals across syllabic contexts , 1977 .

[18]  M. Bush,et al.  Infant place discrimination of voiced stop consonants with and without release bursts , 1977 .

[19]  S. Blumstein,et al.  Perceptual invariance and onset spectra for stop consonants in different vowel environments. , 1980, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.