Does Measuring Body Weight Impact Subsequent Response to Eating Behavior Questions?

Objectives: If being weighed impacts perceptions of eating behavior, it is important that the order of questionnaires and weighing be considered in research and practice. A quasi-experimental study was performed to examine whether being weighed immediately prior to completing a questionnaire affects responses to eating behavior questions. It was hypothesized that being weighed would serve as a priming stimulus and increase measures of dietary restraint, disinhibition, and hunger. Methods: Trained researchers collected a sample of volunteers (n = 355) in 8 locations in the United States on two Saturdays in the summer of 2011. Half of the participants were weighed immediately prior to completing the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ), with the remaining half weighed immediately after TFEQ completion. Results: A priori hypotheses were not supported despite replicating known relationships between weight, dietary restraint and disinhibition. Results indicated that being weighed first produced a difference in differences on disinhibition scores between low restraint score (95% CI = 4.65–6.02) and high restraint score (95% CI = 6.11–7.57) compared to being weighed after questionnaire completion (p = 0.003). However, this relationship was not significant when modeling restraint as a continuous variable, questioning the use of dichotomization. Conclusions: Being weighed is unlikely to be a strong enough prime to significantly change scores on eating behavior questionnaires for everyone, but may allow differences in restraint status to become more evident. Researchers assessing dietary restraint should be wary of the possibility of producing different results when treating restraint as continuous or dichotomous, which could lead to different interpretations.

[1]  R. Wing,et al.  Psychological symptoms in individuals successful at long-term maintenance of weight loss. , 1998, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[2]  A. Acock Working With Missing Values , 2005 .

[3]  A. Stunkard,et al.  The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. , 1985, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[4]  T. Brunner How weight-related cues affect food intake in a modeling situation , 2010, Appetite.

[5]  Michael Siegrist,et al.  Reduced food intake after exposure to subtle weight-related cues , 2012, Appetite.

[6]  B. Gorman,et al.  Some of the most common questions asked of statistical consultants: our favorite responses and recommended readings , 1993 .

[7]  K. Rotenberg,et al.  Effects of priming thoughts about control on anxiety and food intake as moderated by dietary restraint , 2005, Appetite.

[8]  B. Martinson,et al.  Self-weighing promotes weight loss for obese adults. , 2009, American journal of preventive medicine.

[9]  Differential effects of self-weighing in restrained and unrestrained eaters , 2010 .

[10]  P. O'Neil,et al.  Weighing the evidence: benefits of regular weight monitoring for weight control. , 2005, Journal of nutrition education and behavior.

[11]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  www.ssoar.info Why dieters fail: testing the goal conflict model of eating , 2018 .

[12]  Rena R Wing,et al.  International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity Behavioral Self-regulation for Weight Loss in Young Adults: a Randomized Controlled Trial Background , 2022 .

[13]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[14]  Mark A Pereira,et al.  The Impact of Regular Self-weighing on Weight Management: A Systematic Literature Review , 2008, The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity.

[15]  D. Moher,et al.  A comparison of direct vs. self‐report measures for assessing height, weight and body mass index: a systematic review , 2007, Obesity reviews : an official journal of the International Association for the Study of Obesity.

[16]  D. Levitsky,et al.  Monitoring weight daily blocks the freshman weight gain: a model for combating the epidemic of obesity , 2006, International Journal of Obesity.

[17]  R. Engels,et al.  Exposure to slim images in mass media: television commercials as reminders of restriction in restrained eaters. , 2008, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[18]  H. Aarts,et al.  Using stop signals to inhibit chronic dieters' responses toward palatable foods. , 2011, Behaviour research and therapy.

[19]  W. Stroebe,et al.  The allure of forbidden food: On the role of attention in self-regulation , 2008 .

[20]  F. Yeudall,et al.  Monitoring of weight in weight loss programs: a double-edged sword? , 2005, Journal of nutrition education and behavior.

[21]  B. Wansink,et al.  Watching food-related television increases caloric intake in restrained eaters , 2011, Appetite.

[22]  Marcel Zeelenberg,et al.  Flying under the radar: Perverse package size effects on consumption self-regulation , 2008 .

[23]  J. Bargh,et al.  Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype-activation on action. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  E. Papies,et al.  Goal priming and eating behavior: enhancing self-regulation by environmental cues. , 2010, Health psychology : official journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association.

[25]  J. Polivy,et al.  The Effect of Pre-exposure to Food Cues on the Eating Behavior of Restrained and Unrestrained Eaters , 1997, Appetite.

[26]  M. Sevick,et al.  Self-monitoring in weight loss: a systematic review of the literature. , 2011, Journal of the American Dietetic Association.