Effect of Avatar Appearance on Detection Thresholds for Remapped Hand Movements

Hand interaction techniques in virtual reality often exploit visual dominance over proprioception to remap physical hand movements onto different virtual movements. However, when the offset between virtual and physical hands increases, the remapped virtual hand movements are hardly self-attributed, and the users become aware of the remapping. Interestingly, the sense of self-attribution of a body is called the sense of body ownership (SoBO) in the field of psychology, and the realistic the avatar, the stronger is the SoBO. Hence, we hypothesized that realistic avatars (i.e., human hands) can foster self-attribution of the remapped movements better than abstract avatars (i.e., spherical pointers), thus making the remapping less noticeable. In this paper, we present an experiment in which participants repeatedly executed reaching movements with their right hand while different amounts of horizontal shifts were applied. We measured the remapping detection thresholds for each combination of shift directions (left or right) and avatar appearances (realistic or abstract). The results show that realistic avatars increased the detection threshold (i.e., lowered sensitivity) by 31.3% than the abstract avatars when the leftward shift was applied (i.e., when the hand moved in the direction away from the body-midline). In addition, the proprioceptive drift (i.e., the displacement of self-localization toward an avatar) was larger with realistic avatars for leftward shifts, indicating that visual information was given greater preference during visuo-proprioceptive integration in realistic avatars. Our findings quantifiably show that realistic avatars can make remapping less noticeable for larger mismatches between virtual and physical movements and can potentially improve a wide variety of hand-remapping techniques without changing the mapping itself.

[1]  Bruce H. Thomas,et al.  Remapped Physical-Virtual Interfaces with Bimanual Haptic Retargeting , 2019, 2019 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces (VR).

[2]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Virtual Hand Illusion Induced by Visuomotor Correlations , 2010, PloS one.

[3]  Andrea Desantis,et al.  On the influence of causal beliefs on the feeling of agency , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[4]  P. Haggard,et al.  Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action , 2010, Cognition.

[5]  R. Schandry,et al.  Heart beat perception and emotional experience. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[6]  D. Wolpert,et al.  When Feeling Is More Important Than Seeing in Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2002, Current Biology.

[7]  Sophie Jörg,et al.  Need a hand?: how appearance affects the virtual hand illusion , 2016, SAP.

[8]  Massimiliano Di Luca,et al.  The Rubber Hand Illusion: Feeling of Ownership and Proprioceptive Drift Do Not Go Hand in Hand , 2011, PloS one.

[9]  Charles Spence,et al.  When mirrors lie: “Visual capture” of arm position impairs reaching performance , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[10]  Marjolein P. M. Kammers,et al.  The rubber hand illusion in action , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Tomohisa Asai,et al.  Agency elicits body-ownership: proprioceptive drift toward a synchronously acting external proxy , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  H. Critchley,et al.  Knowing your own heart: Distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness , 2015, Biological Psychology.

[13]  Gerd Bruder,et al.  Estimation of Detection Thresholds for Redirected Walking Techniques , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[14]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Effect of Distorted Visual Feedback on the Sense of Agency , 2008, Behavioural neurology.

[15]  M. Martini,et al.  The Bodily Illusion in Adverse Conditions: Virtual Arm Ownership During Visuomotor Mismatch , 2018, Perception.

[16]  P. Haggard,et al.  Having a body versus moving your body: Neural signatures of agency and body-ownership , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[18]  Frederick P. Brooks,et al.  Perceptual sensitivity to visual/kinesthetic discrepancy in hand speed, and why we might care , 2006, VRST '06.

[19]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  Synaesthesia in the normal limb , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  Kazumichi Matsumiya,et al.  Separate multisensory integration processes for ownership and localization of body parts , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[21]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual capture produced by prism spectacles , 1965 .

[22]  Marjolein P. M. Kammers,et al.  What is embodiment? A psychometric approach , 2008, Cognition.

[23]  G. Drew Kessler,et al.  PRISM interaction for enhancing control in immersive virtual environments , 2007, TCHI.

[24]  Eyal Ofek,et al.  Sparse Haptic Proxy: Touch Feedback in Virtual Environments Using a General Passive Prop , 2017, CHI.

[25]  D. Lloyd Spatial limits on referred touch to an alien limb may reflect boundaries of visuo-tactile peripersonal space surrounding the hand , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[26]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  Hands only illusion: multisensory integration elicits sense of ownership for body parts but not for non-corporeal objects , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  Thomas Goschke,et al.  Unconscious modulation of the conscious experience of voluntary control , 2007, Cognition.

[28]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Mel Slater,et al.  First Person Perspective of Seated Participants Over a Walking Virtual Body Leads to Illusory Agency Over the Walking , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[30]  P. Haggard,et al.  Sense of agency , 2012, Current Biology.

[31]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  Experimenting with the acting self , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[32]  M. Leek Adaptive procedures in psychophysical research , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[33]  Eric Burns,et al.  Combining passive haptics with redirected walking , 2005, ICAT '05.

[34]  Sharif Razzaque,et al.  Redirected Walking , 2001, Eurographics.

[35]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Defective recognition of one's own actions in patients with schizophrenia. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[36]  D. Wegner,et al.  Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will. , 1999, The American psychologist.

[37]  Tabitha C. Peck,et al.  Avatar Embodiment. Towards a Standardized Questionnaire , 2018, Front. Robot. AI.

[38]  Mel Slater,et al.  The Sense of Embodiment in Virtual Reality , 2012, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[39]  Ferran Argelaguet,et al.  The role of interaction in virtual embodiment: Effects of the virtual hand representation , 2016, 2016 IEEE Virtual Reality (VR).

[40]  M. Wiesmann,et al.  Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. , 2008, Eating behaviors.

[41]  Takuji Narumi,et al.  Modifying an identified curved surface shape using pseudo-haptic effect , 2012, 2012 IEEE Haptics Symposium (HAPTICS).

[42]  Mel Slater,et al.  Seeing an Embodied Virtual Hand is Analgesic Contingent on Colocation. , 2017, The journal of pain : official journal of the American Pain Society.

[43]  H. Critchley,et al.  Multisensory integration across exteroceptive and interoceptive domains modulates self-experience in the rubber-hand illusion , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Sharif Razzaque,et al.  The Hand Is More Easily Fooled than the Eye: Users Are More Sensitive to Visual Interpenetration than to Visual-Proprioceptive Discrepancy , 2006, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[45]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Over my fake body: body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[46]  Takuji Narumi,et al.  Virtual Hand Realism Affects Object Size Perception in Body-Based Scaling , 2019, 2019 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces (VR).

[47]  A. Yamashita,et al.  The influence of performance on action-effect integration in sense of agency , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.

[48]  Joan López-Moliner,et al.  The Effects of Visuomotor Calibration to the Perceived Space and Body, through Embodiment in Immersive Virtual Reality , 2015, TAP.

[49]  Catherine Preston,et al.  The role of distance from the body and distance from the real hand in ownership and disownership during the rubber hand illusion. , 2013, Acta psychologica.

[50]  Eric D. Ragan,et al.  Evaluating Remapped Physical Reach for Hand Interactions with Passive Haptics in Virtual Reality , 2018, IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics.

[51]  M. Ernst,et al.  Optimal integration of shape information from vision and touch , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  Mary C. Whitton,et al.  Redirected touching: The effect of warping space on task performance , 2012, 2012 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI).

[53]  I ROCK,et al.  Vision and Touch: An Experimentally Created Conflict between the Two Senses , 1964, Science.

[54]  W. IJsselsteijn,et al.  The effect of similarities in skin texture and hand shape on perceived ownership of a fake limb. , 2008, Body image.

[55]  Mel Slater,et al.  Body ownership causes illusory self-attribution of speaking and influences subsequent real speaking , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[56]  Jörg Müller,et al.  Ownershift: Facilitating Overhead Interaction in Virtual Reality with an Ownership-Preserving Hand Space Shift , 2018, UIST.

[57]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[58]  Sean Follmer,et al.  Visuo-Haptic Illusions for Improving the Perceived Performance of Shape Displays , 2018, CHI.

[59]  Sean Follmer,et al.  A Functional Optimization Based Approach for Continuous 3D Retargeted Touch of Arbitrary, Complex Boundaries in Haptic Virtual Reality , 2018, CHI.

[60]  S. Debener,et al.  The Senses of Agency and Ownership: A Review , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[61]  Torsten Ingemann Nielsen,et al.  VOLITION: A NEW EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH , 1963 .

[62]  Frank Weichert,et al.  Analysis of the Accuracy and Robustness of the Leap Motion Controller , 2013, Sensors.

[63]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  Behavioural Brain Research , 1999 .

[64]  Dongjun Lee,et al.  Enlarging just noticeable differences of visual-proprioceptive conflict in VR using haptic feedback , 2015, 2015 IEEE World Haptics Conference (WHC).

[65]  Ye Yuan,et al.  Is the rubber hand illusion induced by immersive virtual reality? , 2010, 2010 IEEE Virtual Reality Conference (VR).

[66]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[67]  C. Moore,et al.  Movement and the Rubber Hand Illusion , 2009, Perception.

[68]  Mel Slater,et al.  Is my hand connected to my body? The impact of body continuity and arm alignment on the virtual hand illusion , 2011, Cognitive Neurodynamics.

[69]  Eyal Ofek,et al.  Haptic Retargeting: Dynamic Repurposing of Passive Haptics for Enhanced Virtual Reality Experiences , 2016, CHI.

[70]  Charles Spence,et al.  Reaching with alien limbs: Visual exposure to prosthetic hands in a mirror biases proprioception without accompanying illusions of ownership , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[71]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[72]  Manos Tsakiris,et al.  Just a heartbeat away from one's body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[73]  James W. Moore,et al.  What Is the Sense of Agency and Why Does it Matter? , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[74]  Valentin Schwind,et al.  Touch with foreign hands: the effect of virtual hand appearance on visual-haptic integration , 2018, SAP.

[75]  H. Henrik Ehrsson,et al.  The spatial distance rule in the moving and classical rubber hand illusions , 2014, Consciousness and Cognition.

[76]  Sriram Subramanian,et al.  Erg-O: Ergonomic Optimization of Immersive Virtual Environments , 2017, UIST.

[77]  Ronan Boulic,et al.  Self-attribution of distorted reaching movements in immersive virtual reality , 2018, Comput. Graph..

[78]  Tovi Grossman,et al.  The bubble cursor: enhancing target acquisition by dynamic resizing of the cursor's activation area , 2005, CHI.

[79]  Anatole Lécuyer,et al.  Simulating Haptic Feedback Using Vision: A Survey of Research and Applications of Pseudo-Haptic Feedback , 2009, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[80]  Antonio Krüger,et al.  Estimating Detection Thresholds for Desktop-Scale Hand Redirection in Virtual Reality , 2019, 2019 IEEE Conference on Virtual Reality and 3D User Interfaces (VR).

[81]  Andrew J. Doxon,et al.  Peri-personal space as a prior in coupling visual and proprioceptive signals , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[82]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Visual-proprioceptive interaction under large amounts of conflict. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[83]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[84]  D. Wegner,et al.  Modulating the sense of agency with external cues , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.