Bounded rationality in C. elegans is explained by circuit-specific normalization in chemosensory pathways

Rational choice theory assumes optimality in decision-making. Violations of a basic axiom of economic rationality known as “Independence of Irrelevant Alternatives” (IIA) have been demonstrated in both humans and animals and could stem from common neuronal constraints. Here we develop tests for IIA in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans, an animal with only 302 neurons, using olfactory chemotaxis assays. We find that in most cases C. elegans make rational decisions. However, by probing multiple neuronal architectures using various choice sets, we show that violations of rationality arise when the circuit of olfactory sensory neurons is asymmetric. We further show that genetic manipulations of the asymmetry between the AWC neurons can make the worm irrational. Last, a context-dependent normalization-based model of value coding and gain control explains how particular neuronal constraints on information coding give rise to irrationality. Thus, we demonstrate that bounded rationality could arise due to basic neuronal constraints. Innate odor preferences in C. elegans are controlled by the activation of a pair of olfactory sensory neurons. Here, the authors show that asymmetric activation of the AWCON and AWCO FF neurons can lead to irrational olfactory preferences that are explained by a model of normalization of sensory gain control.

[1]  K. Touhara,et al.  [A molecular basis for odorant recognition: olfactory receptor pharmacology]. , 2004, Nihon yakurigaku zasshi. Folia pharmacologica Japonica.

[2]  A. Tversky,et al.  Context-dependent preferences , 1993 .

[3]  P. Samuelson A Note on the Pure Theory of Consumer's Behaviour , 1938 .

[4]  S. Stearns Daniel Bernoulli (1738): evolution and economics under risk , 2000, Journal of Biosciences.

[5]  J. Neumann,et al.  Theory of games and economic behavior , 1945, 100 Years of Math Milestones.

[6]  Ryan Webb,et al.  Adaptive neural coding: from biological to behavioral decision-making , 2015, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[7]  M. Félix,et al.  The Natural Biotic Environment of Caenorhabditis elegans , 2017, Genetics.

[8]  Paul W Sternberg,et al.  Communication between oocytes and somatic cells regulates volatile pheromone production in Caenorhabditis elegans , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Theresa Stiernagle Maintenance of C. elegans. , 2006, WormBook : the online review of C. elegans biology.

[10]  John M. Walker,et al.  C. elegans , 2006, Methods in Molecular Biology.

[11]  Cori Bargmann,et al.  C. elegans odour discrimination requires asymmetric diversity in olfactory neurons , 2001, Nature.

[12]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  Natural selection can favour ‘irrational’ behaviour , 2014, Biology Letters.

[13]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Reward Value-Based Gain Control: Divisive Normalization in Parietal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect Theory : An Analysis of Decision under Risk Author ( s ) : , 2007 .

[15]  H. Simon,et al.  Rational choice and the structure of the environment. , 1956, Psychological review.

[16]  Quantifying male and female pheromone-based mate choice in Caenorhabditis nematodes using a novel microfluidic technique , 2017, bioRxiv.

[17]  R. Axel Scents and Sensibility: A Molecular Logic of Olfactory Perception (Nobel Lecture) , 2005 .

[18]  Erik M. Jorgensen,et al.  The Sensory Circuitry for Sexual Attraction in C. elegans Males , 2007, Current Biology.

[19]  Netta Cohen,et al.  Neural Architecture of Hunger-Dependent Multisensory Decision Making in C. elegans , 2016, Neuron.

[20]  S. Ward Chemotaxis by the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans: identification of attractants and analysis of the response by use of mutants. , 1973, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  Travis A. Jarrell,et al.  The Connectome of a Decision-Making Neural Network , 2012, Science.

[22]  A. Zaslaver,et al.  Irrational behavior in C. elegans arises from asymmetric modulatory effects within single sensory neurons , 2019, Nature Communications.

[23]  H. Simon,et al.  A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice , 1955 .

[24]  Cori Bargmann,et al.  Odorant-selective genes and neurons mediate olfaction in C. elegans , 1993, Cell.

[25]  S. Afriat THE CONSTRUCTION OF UTILITY FUNCTIONS FROM EXPENDITURE DATA , 1967 .

[26]  Cori Bargmann,et al.  A natural variant and engineered mutation in a GPCR promote DEET resistance in C. elegans , 2018, Nature.

[27]  R. Axel,et al.  A novel multigene family may encode odorant receptors: A molecular basis for odor recognition , 1991, Cell.

[28]  A. R. Palmer,et al.  From symmetry to asymmetry: phylogenetic patterns of asymmetry variation in animals and their evolutionary significance. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  Elizabeth E Glater,et al.  Identification of Odor Blend Used by Caenorhabditis elegans for Pathogen Recognition , 2018, Chemical senses.

[30]  W. T. Nickell,et al.  Single Ionic Channels of Two Caenorhabditis elegans Chemosensory Neurons in Native Membrane , 2002, The Journal of Membrane Biology.

[31]  J. Hodgkin What does a worm want with 20,000 genes? , 2001, Genome Biology.

[32]  Leonid Kruglyak,et al.  Catecholamine receptor polymorphisms affect decision-making in C. elegans , 2011, Nature.

[33]  Mel W. Khaw,et al.  Normalization is a general neural mechanism for context-dependent decision making , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Efficient Coding and the Neural Representation of Value , 2022 .

[35]  Cori Bargmann,et al.  Signal transduction in the Caenorhabditis elegans nervous system. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[36]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision Making and Sequential Sampling from Memory , 2016, Neuron.

[37]  T. Andrew Hurly,et al.  Context-dependent, risk-sensitive foraging preferences in wild rufous hummingbirds , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  K. Doya Modulators of decision making , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  T. A. Hurly,et al.  Context–dependent foraging decisions in rufous hummingbirds , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  A. Rangel,et al.  Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[41]  C. Chuang,et al.  Stochastic left–right neuronal asymmetry in Caenorhabditis elegans , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  Cori Bargmann Chemosensation in C. elegans. , 2006, WormBook : the online review of C. elegans biology.

[43]  S. Shafir Intransitivity of preferences in honey bees: support for 'comparative' evaluation of foraging options , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[44]  S. Shafir,et al.  Context-dependent violations of rational choice in honeybees (Apis mellifera) and gray jays (Perisoreus canadensis) , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[45]  A. Tversky,et al.  Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty , 1992 .

[46]  S. Lockery,et al.  Neuronal microcircuits for decision making in C. elegans , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[47]  Cori Bargmann,et al.  Alternative olfactory neuron fates are specified by the LIM homeobox gene lim-4. , 1999, Genes & development.

[48]  R. Rogers The Roles of Dopamine and Serotonin in Decision Making: Evidence from Pharmacological Experiments in Humans , 2011, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[49]  Hiroshi Yamada,et al.  Thirst-dependent risk preferences in monkeys identify a primitive form of wealth , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  Oliver Hobert,et al.  Two distinct types of neuronal asymmetries are controlled by the Caenorhabditis elegans zinc finger transcription factor die-1 , 2014, Genes & development.

[51]  Erin L. Rich,et al.  Decoding subjective decisions from orbitofrontal cortex , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  N. Metcalfe,et al.  Context-dependent mate choice in relation to social composition in green swordtails Xiphophorus helleri , 2008 .

[53]  Jae Im Choi,et al.  Odor-dependent temporal dynamics in Caenorhabitis elegans adaptation and aversive learning behavior , 2018, PeerJ.

[54]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Individual Choice Behavior: A Theoretical Analysis , 1979 .

[55]  A. Barrios,et al.  Exploratory decisions of the Caenorhabditis elegans male: a conflict of two drives. , 2014, Seminars in cell & developmental biology.

[56]  P. Glimcher,et al.  Rationalizing Context-Dependent Preferences: Divisive Normalization and Neurobiological Constraints on Choice , 2016 .