Spatial Learning: Conditions and Basic Effects

A growing body of evidence suggests that the spatial and the temporal domains seem to share the same or similar conditions, basic effects, and mechanisms. The blocking, unblocking and overshadowing experiments (and also those of latent inhibition and perceptual learning reviewed by Prados and Redhead in this issue) show that to exclude associative learning as a basic mechanism responsible for spatial learning is quite inappropriate. All these results, especially those obtained with strictly spatial tasks, seem inconsistent with O’Keefe and Nadel’s account of true spatial learning or locale learning. Their theory claims that this kind of learning is fundamentally different and develops with total independence from other ways of learning (like classical and instrumental conditioning -taxon learning). In fact, the results reviewed can be explained appealing on to a sophisticated guidance system, like for example the one proposed by Leonard and McNaughton (1990; see also McNaughton and cols, 1996). Such a system would allow that an animal generates new space information: given the distance and address from of A to B and from A to C, being able to infer the distance and the address from B to C, even when C is invisible from B (see Chapuis and Varlet, 1987 -the contribution by McLaren in this issue constitutes a good example of a sophisticated guidance system).

[1]  N. J. Mackintosh,et al.  Blocking and Overshadowing between Intra-Maze and Extra-Maze Cues: A Test of the Independence of Locale and Guidance Learning , 1985 .

[2]  J. Pearce,et al.  CONTROL OF SPATIAL BEHAVIOR BY AN UNSTABLE LANDMARK , 1998 .

[3]  E. Tolman,et al.  Studies in spatial learning; place learning versus response learning. , 1946, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Spatial generalization in honeybees confirms Shepard's law , 1999, Behavioural Processes.

[5]  T. Alloway,et al.  Food Aversion Learning , 1977 .

[6]  S. Smith‐Roe,et al.  Spatial learning and performance in the radial arm maze is impaired after N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor blockade in striatal subregions. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[7]  John M. Pearce,et al.  Aprendizaje y cognición , 1998 .

[8]  M. Bitterman Comparative analysis of learning in honeybees , 1996 .

[9]  V. D. Chamizo,et al.  Reciprocal Overshadowing between Intra-Maze and Extra-Maze Cues , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[10]  R Biegler,et al.  Blocking in the spatial domain with arrays of discrete landmarks. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[11]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Hippocampal place units in the freely moving rat: Why they fire where they fire , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Pearce Similarity and discrimination: a selective review and a connectionist model. , 1994, Psychological review.

[14]  V. D. Chamizo,et al.  Latent learning and latent inhibition in maze discriminations , 1989 .

[15]  M. A. Saavedra Pavlovian compound conditioning in the rabbit , 1975 .

[16]  John M. Pearce,et al.  Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on cognitive maps but not heading vectors , 1998, Nature.

[17]  Normal Learning Set and Facilitation of Reversal Learning in Rats with Combined Fornix‐Amygdala Lesions: Implications for Preserved Learning Abilities in Amnesia a , 1985, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  A. Dickinson Contemporary Animal Learning Theory , 1981 .

[19]  Gurindar S. Sohi,et al.  Memory systems , 1996, CSUR.

[20]  J. López,et al.  Place and cue learning in turtles , 2000 .

[21]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Hippocampectomized rats are capable of homing by path integration. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  R. Hinde Constraints on learning , 1973 .

[23]  J. Pearce,et al.  A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980 .

[24]  A. Black,et al.  Stimulus control of spatial behavior on the eight-arm maze in rats ☆ ☆☆ , 1980 .

[25]  Second-order conditioning of the pigeon's key-peck using an autoshaping procedure. , 1980, The American journal of psychology.

[26]  H. Eichenbaum The Hippocampal System and Declarative Memory in Animals , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  Marcia L. Spetch,et al.  Pigeons′ Use of Landmarks Presented in Digitized Images , 1994 .

[28]  R. Sutherland,et al.  A behavioural analysis of spatial localization following electrolytic, kainate- or colchicine-induced damage to the hippocampal formation in the rat , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  R. Morris Spatial Localization Does Not Require the Presence of Local Cues , 1981 .

[30]  W E Skaggs,et al.  Deciphering the hippocampal polyglot: the hippocampus as a path integration system. , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[31]  N. Guttman,et al.  Discriminability and stimulus generalization. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.

[32]  S. Revusky Chapter 4 – THE ROLE OF INTERFERENCE IN ASSOCIATION OVER A DELAY1 , 1971 .

[33]  S. Revusky,et al.  Learning as a General Process with an Emphasis on Data from Feeding Experiments , 1977 .

[34]  R Biegler,et al.  Landmark Stability: Further Studies Pointing to a Role in Spatial Learning , 1996, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[35]  R. Morris,et al.  Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions , 1982, Nature.

[36]  J. Prados,et al.  Locating an invisible goal in a water maze requires at least two landmarks , 1998, Psychobiology.

[37]  B. A. Cartwright,et al.  How honey bees use landmarks to guide their return to a food source , 1982, Nature.

[38]  P. Holland,et al.  Differential effects of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after Pavlovian appetitive conditioning. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[39]  William T. Keeton,et al.  The Orientational and Navigational Basis of Homing in Birds , 1974 .

[40]  I. Whishaw Latent learning in a Swimming Pool Place Task by Rats: Evidence for the use of Associative and not Cognitive Mapping Processes , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[41]  C. L. Hull Principles of Behavior , 1945 .

[42]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[43]  Pearce Animal learning and cognition , 1997 .

[44]  W. K. Honig,et al.  Handbook of Operant Behavior , 2022 .

[45]  G. M. Josin,et al.  Response learning , 1990, 1990 IJCNN International Joint Conference on Neural Networks.

[46]  K Cheng,et al.  Shepard's Universal Law Supported by Honeybees in Spatial Generalization , 2000, Psychological science.

[47]  I. Gormezano,et al.  CONDITIONING OF THE NICTITATING MEMBRANE OF THE RABBIT AS A FUNCTION OF CS-US INTERVAL. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[48]  H. M. Hanson Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[49]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Hippocampal system dysfunction and odor discrimination learning in rats: impairment or facilitation depending on representational demands. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[50]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Dissociation of Mechanisms Involved in Dogs’ Oriented Displacements , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[51]  J Ward-Robinson,et al.  Influence of a beacon on spatial learning based on the shape of the test environment. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[52]  B. Skinner,et al.  Principles of Behavior , 1944 .

[53]  G. Lynch,et al.  Hippocampus and olfactory discrimination learning: effects of entorhinal cortex lesions on olfactory learning and memory in a successive-cue, go-no-go task. , 1991, Behavioral neuroscience.

[54]  C. H. Honzik,et al.  Degrees of hunger, reward and non-reward, and maze learning in rats, and Introduction and removal of reward, and maze performance in rats , 1930 .

[55]  J. Pearce,et al.  Interaction between piloting and beacon homing by rats in a swimming pool. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[56]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[57]  John Garcia,et al.  Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning , 1966 .

[58]  P. Rozin,et al.  Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. , 1971, Psychological review.

[59]  A. G. Baker,et al.  Excitatory and inhibitory conditioning following uncorrelated presentations of CS and UCS , 1977 .

[60]  C. Mark Leyland,et al.  Higher Order Autoshaping* , 1977 .

[61]  N. Mackintosh Blocking of conditioned suppression: role of the first compound trial. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[62]  S. T. The use of visual landmarks by gerbils : Reaching a goal when landmarks are displaced , .

[63]  F. Restle Discrimination of cues in mazes: a resolution of the place-vs.-response question. , 1957, Psychological review.

[64]  R. D. Manteiga,et al.  Aprendizaje elemental a pesar de entrenamiento configuracional en una tarea de navegación , 2001 .

[65]  B. Campbell,et al.  Punishment and aversive behavior , 1969 .

[66]  N. Chapuis,et al.  Short Cuts by Dogs in Natural Surroundings , 1987 .

[67]  L. Kamin Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning , 1967 .

[68]  N. Mackintosh Overshadowing and stimulus intensity , 1976, Animal learning & behavior.

[69]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Conditioning And Associative Learning , 1983 .

[70]  V. D. Chamizo,et al.  Overshadowing in the spatial domain , 1999 .

[71]  N. Mackintosh The psychology of animal learning , 1974 .

[72]  R. Sutherland,et al.  A characterization of performance by men and women in a virtual Morris water task: A large and reliable sex difference , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.

[73]  J. Pearce A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning. , 1987, Psychological review.

[74]  J. Pearce,et al.  Blocking in the Morris swimming pool. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[75]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[76]  G. Cicala,et al.  Blocking of conditioned suppression with 1 or 10 compound trials , 1986 .

[77]  E. Tolman,et al.  The organism and the causal texture of the environment. , 1935 .

[78]  R. A. Koelling,et al.  Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. , 1955, Science.

[79]  M L Spetch,et al.  Learning the configuration of a landmark array: I. Touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans. , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[80]  T. S. Collett,et al.  Landmark learning in bees , 1983, Journal of comparative physiology.

[81]  N. J. Mackintosh,et al.  Stimulus selection: Learning to ignore stimuli that predict no change in reinforcement. , 1973 .

[82]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Configural association theory: The role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia , 1989, Psychobiology.

[83]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Spatial representation in the rat: Conceptual, behavioral, and neurophysiological perspectives , 1990 .

[84]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[85]  H. Blodgett,et al.  Relative strength of place and response learning in the T maze. , 1948, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[86]  Bryan Kolb,et al.  Spatial mapping: definitive disruption by hippocampal or medial frontal cortical damage in the rat , 1982, Neuroscience Letters.

[87]  K. Cheng The vector sum model of pigeon landmark use. , 1989 .

[88]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans , 1996, Nature.

[89]  Derek A. Hamilton,et al.  Blocking in human place learning: Evidence from virtual navigation , 1999, Psychobiology.

[90]  M L Spetch,et al.  Overshadowing in landmark learning: touch-screen studies with pigeons and humans. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[91]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Single unit activity in the rat hippocampus during a spatial memory task , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[92]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[93]  F. W. Irwin Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men , 1932, The Psychological Clinic.

[94]  R. Biegler,et al.  Landmark stability is a prerequisite for spatial but not discrimination learning , 1993, Nature.

[95]  M. Seligman,et al.  Biological boundaries of learning. , 1972 .

[96]  C Beaulieu,et al.  Spatial encoding of hidden objects in dogs (Canis familiaris). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[97]  S. Healy Spatial representation in animals. , 1998 .

[98]  V. D. Chamizo,et al.  Blocking in the spatial domain. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[99]  Marcia L. Spetch,et al.  Blocking in landmark-based search in honeybees , 2001 .

[100]  Marcia L. Spetch,et al.  Spatial peak shift and generalization in pigeons. , 1997 .

[101]  D. Olton,et al.  Neurobiology of Comparative Cognition , 1990 .

[102]  Sharon R. Doerkson,et al.  Use of Landmark Configuration in Pigeons and Humans : II . Generality Across Search Tasks , 2001 .

[103]  S. Shettleworth Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .