The Eyes as Window to the Mind.

In this issue,Moriuchi et al. report on a fascinating study (1) on a long-standing question: Why do children with autism look less at other people’s eyes, relative to typically developing children? The authors pit two theories against each other: gaze aversion and gaze indifference. Their results point to the latter, not the former, although the question of why remains. One now quite old idea is that children with autism don’t see other people’s eyes as a window to the mind (2). Their well-established difficulties in attributing mental states to other people may mean that they see other people’s eyes as parts within the face that move, but they struggle to interpret what thesemoving partsmean.Whereas the typical child pays a lot of attention to where others are looking because they understand that what someone looks at indicates their mental state (what that person is interested in), children with autism maynot appreciate the mentalistic significance of a person’s eyes. Nor might people with autism understand that a person’s eyes are a reflection of a range of mental states—cognitive, volitional, or affective. Considerhowwhenwe try to interpret a person’s basic emotions, such as happy, sad, angry, afraid, disgust, or surprise (Figure 1A), people with or without autism score equallywell if they are presentedwith the eyes or the mouth, although seeing the whole face leads to better performance. But when the task is to judge a complex emotion, such as arrogant, guilt, thoughtful, or revenge (Figure 1B), seeing the eyes alone results in as good performance as seeing the whole face, and both eyes and whole face are better than the mouth alone. Moreover, on such challenging tests, adults with Asperger’s syndrome show impaired performance when shown just the eyes. Using the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test, collecting data online, our group found that the impairment in autism is very clear, as is the sex difference (female superiority) in typical participants and the absence of any sex difference in autism (4), confirming earlier studies (5, 6) (Figure 2). Performance on the Eyes test is associated with activity in the amygdala and in the left inferior frontal gyrus (7, 8); the female advantage on the test is associated with greater activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus, and the autistic deficit is associated with reduced activity in this same area. This deficit is also seen in parents of such children, suggesting that it arises for genetic reasons (9, 10). Lesion studies confirm the importance of these brain regions in mentalizing the eyes (11). Candidate gene studies suggest that sex steroid pathway genes play a role in performance on the Eyes test (12), and a FIGURE 1. Examples of Basic and Complex Emotional Stimuli

[1]  T. Bourgeron,et al.  Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions and brain anatomy , 2016, bioRxiv.

[2]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Parents of Children with Asperger Syndrome: What is the Cognitive Phenotype? , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Genes related to sex steroids, neural growth, and social–emotional behavior are associated with autistic traits, empathy, and Asperger syndrome , 2009, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[4]  Michael V. Lombardo,et al.  The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test: Complete Absence of Typical Sex Difference in ~400 Men and Women with Autism , 2015, PloS one.

[5]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[6]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. , 1997, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[7]  Ian B. Hickie,et al.  Intranasal Oxytocin Improves Emotion Recognition for Youth with Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[8]  Shir Tragash,et al.  reading the mind in the eyes , 2018 .

[9]  Tony Charman,et al.  Infant Neural Sensitivity to Dynamic Eye Gaze Is Associated with Later Emerging Autism , 2012, Current Biology.

[10]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism , 1989 .

[11]  D. Schutter,et al.  Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. , 2001, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[13]  Warren Jones,et al.  Mechanisms of Diminished Attention to Eyes in Autism. , 2017, The American journal of psychiatry.

[14]  Rebecca C. Knickmeyer,et al.  Fetal testosterone and empathy: Evidence from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[15]  E. Bullmore,et al.  fMRI of parents of children with Asperger Syndrome: A pilot study , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[16]  R. Morris,et al.  Differential Effects of Lesions of the Amygdala and Prefrontal Cortex on Recognizing Facial Expressions of Complex Emotions , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Testosterone reduces functional connectivity during the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ Test , 2016, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[18]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Is There a "Language of the Eyes"? Evidence from Normal Adults, and Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome , 1997 .