Around the world, adolescence is a time of heightened sensation seeking and immature self-regulation.

The dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking portrays the period as one characterized by a combination of heightened sensation seeking and still-maturing self-regulation, but most tests of this model have been conducted in the United States or Western Europe. In the present study, these propositions are tested in an international sample of more than 5000 individuals between ages 10 and 30 years from 11 countries in Africa, Asia, Europe and the Americas, using a multi-method test battery that includes both self-report and performance-based measures of both constructs. Consistent with the dual systems model, sensation seeking increased between preadolescence and late adolescence, peaked at age 19, and declined thereafter, whereas self-regulation increased steadily from preadolescence into young adulthood, reaching a plateau between ages 23 and 26. Although there were some variations in the magnitude of the observed age trends, the developmental patterns were largely similar across countries.

[1]  D. Wechsler Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence--Second Edition , 2018 .

[2]  Melanie R. Silverman,et al.  When Is an Adolescent an Adult? Assessing Cognitive Control in Emotional and Nonemotional Contexts , 2016, Psychological science.

[3]  Ashley R. Smith,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience the Dual Systems Model: Review, Reappraisal, and Reaffirmation , 2022 .

[4]  Adriana Galván,et al.  Beyond simple models of adolescence to an integrated circuit-based account: A commentary , 2015, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Sabine Peters,et al.  The link between testosterone and amygdala–orbitofrontal cortex connectivity in adolescent alcohol use , 2015, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[6]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Unpacking Self-Control. , 2015, Child development perspectives.

[7]  B. Casey Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[8]  Nienke Meulman,et al.  An ERP study on L2 syntax processing: When do learners fail? , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[9]  Bengt Muthén,et al.  IRT studies of many groups: the alignment method , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[10]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis of reward-related brain activation: Effects of age, pubertal stage, and reward sensitivity , 2014, Brain and Cognition.

[11]  J. Jolles,et al.  Changes in neural mechanisms of cognitive control during the transition from late adolescence to young adulthood , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Ashley R. Smith,et al.  Impact of socio-emotional context, brain development, and pubertal maturation on adolescent risk-taking , 2013, Hormones and Behavior.

[13]  Monica Luciana,et al.  Incentive Motivation, Cognitive Control, and the Adolescent Brain: Is It Time for a Paradigm Shift? , 2012, Child development perspectives.

[14]  K. Harden,et al.  Differential changes in impulsivity and sensation seeking and the escalation of substance use from adolescence to early adulthood , 2012, Development and Psychopathology.

[15]  Nicholas B. Allen,et al.  Arrested development? Reconsidering dual-systems models of brain function in adolescence and disorders , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  James N. Porter,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of incentive motivation in adolescence: age-related changes in signaling, individual differences, and implications for the development of self-regulation. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[17]  Todd A. Hare,et al.  Frontostriatal Maturation Predicts Cognitive Control Failure to Appetitive Cues in Adolescents , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  E. Claus,et al.  Cognitive Control in Adolescence: Neural Underpinnings and Relation to Self-Report Behaviors , 2011, PloS one.

[19]  E. Tucker-Drob,et al.  Individual differences in the development of sensation seeking and impulsivity during adolescence: further evidence for a dual systems model. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[20]  G. Hofstede Dimensionalizing cultures: The Hofstede model in context , 2011 .

[21]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Parenting Attributions and Attitudes in Diverse Cultural Contexts: Introduction to the Special Issue , 2011 .

[22]  L. Steinberg,et al.  Judgment and Decision Making in Adolescence , 2011 .

[23]  Serge A. R. B. Rombouts,et al.  Adolescent risky decision-making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions , 2010, NeuroImage.

[24]  S. Erkut Developing Multiple Language Versions of Instruments for Intercultural Research. , 2010, Child development perspectives.

[25]  A. Shotar,et al.  Knowledge of Substance Abuse Among High School Students in Jordan , 2010, Journal of transcultural nursing : official journal of the Transcultural Nursing Society.

[26]  R. Larson,et al.  Globalization, Societal Change, and Adolescence Across the World , 2009 .

[27]  M. Banich,et al.  Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[28]  L. Steinberg A Social Neuroscience Perspective on Adolescent Risk-Taking. , 2008, Developmental review : DR.

[29]  T. Hare,et al.  The Adolescent Brain , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[30]  E. Claus,et al.  Brain activation during the Stroop task in adolescents with severe substance and conduct problems: A pilot study. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[31]  Maristela Monteiro,et al.  The Global Distribution of Average Volume of Alcohol Consumption and Patterns of Drinking , 2003, European Addiction Research.

[32]  Paul Slovic,et al.  The Springs of Action: Affective and Analytical Information Processing in Choice , 2000 .

[33]  Grace Palladino Teenagers: An American History , 1996 .

[34]  J. Weisz,et al.  A multimethod study of problem behavior among Thai and American children in school: teacher reports versus direct observations. , 1995, Child development.

[35]  M. Zuckerman Behavioral Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking , 1994 .

[36]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[37]  L. Steinberg,et al.  Authoritative parenting and adolescent adjustment across varied ecological niches. , 1990 .

[38]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[39]  L. Steinberg,et al.  Sex Differences in the Developmental Trajectories of Impulse Control and Sensation-Seeking from Early Adolescence to Early Adulthood , 2014, Journal of Youth and Adolescence.

[40]  L. Spear,et al.  Adolescent neurodevelopment. , 2013, The Journal of adolescent health : official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine.

[41]  E. Claus,et al.  Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[42]  Gert Jan Hofstede,et al.  Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind, 3rd ed. , 2010 .

[43]  Laurence Steinberg,et al.  Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. , 2009, Child development.

[44]  Xinyin Chen,et al.  Social functioning and adjustment in Chinese children: the imprint of historical time. , 2005, Child development.

[45]  Tony Addison,et al.  Human Development Report, 1990.@@@Human Development Report, 1991. , 1993 .