Complex interactions between spatial, orientation, and motion cues for biological motion perception across visual space.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Vogels,et al. Functional differentiation of macaque visual temporal cortical neurons using a parametric action space. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.
[2] R. Blake,et al. What constitutes an efficient reference frame for vision? , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[3] Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel,et al. Signature Movements Lead to Efficient Search for Threatening Actions , 2012, PloS one.
[4] J. Anthony Movshon,et al. Dissociation of Neuronal and Psychophysical Responses to Local and Global Motion , 2011, Current Biology.
[5] D. Atchison,et al. MRI study of the changes in crystalline lens shape with accommodation and aging in humans. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[6] Luciano Fadiga,et al. Interaction of visual hemifield and body view in biological motion perception , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.
[7] Rick Gurnsey,et al. Limits of peripheral direction discrimination of point-light walkers. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[8] H E M. Journal of Neurophysiology , 1938, Nature.
[9] J Zhang,et al. Motion contrast and motion integration. , 1993, Vision research.
[10] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[11] Bruce C Hansen,et al. Peripheral vision: good for biological motion, bad for signal noise segregation? , 2007, Journal of vision.
[12] T. Poggio,et al. Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[13] R. L. Valois,et al. Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors , 1991, Vision Research.
[14] G. Orban,et al. Human Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Reveals Separation and Integration of Shape and Motion Cues in Biological Motion Processing , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[15] M. Lappe,et al. Perception of biological motion from limited-lifetime stimuli , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[16] Sheng He,et al. Emergent Filling In Induced by Motion Integration Reveals a High-Level Mechanism in Filling In , 2012, Psychological science.
[17] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. Peripheral sensitivity to biological motion conveyed by first and second-order signals , 2010, Vision Research.
[18] S. Nishida,et al. Adaptive pooling of visual motion signals by the human visual system revealed with a novel multi-element stimulus. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[19] Emily D Grossman,et al. Temporal "Bubbles" reveal key features for point-light biological motion perception. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[20] D Marr,et al. Directional selectivity and its use in early visual processing , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[21] Antonino Casile,et al. Critical features for the recognition of biological motion. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[22] R. Blake,et al. Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[23] Stuart Anstis,et al. The furrow illusion: peripheral motion becomes aligned with stationary contours. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[24] George Mather,et al. Motion-induced position shifts occur after motion integration , 2009, Vision Research.
[25] G. Orban,et al. Specificity of regions processing biological motion , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.
[26] Yi Jiang,et al. Life motion signals lengthen perceived temporal duration , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[27] M. Sereno,et al. Point-Light Biological Motion Perception Activates Human Premotor Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] K. D. Valois,et al. Motion contrast and motion integration , 1993, Vision Research.
[29] J. Lange,et al. A Model of Biological Motion Perception from Configural Form Cues , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[30] E. Adelson,et al. Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.
[31] M. Shiffrar,et al. Subconfigurations of the human form in the perception of biological motion displays. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[32] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[33] Hongjing Lu,et al. Structural processing in biological motion perception. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[34] N. Troje,et al. The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? , 2006, Current Biology.
[35] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[36] H. Lu,et al. Visual search by action category. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[37] M. Pavlova,et al. Orientation specificity in biological motion perception , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[38] G. Mather,et al. Low-level visual processing of biological motion , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[39] P. Cavanagh,et al. Attention-based visual routines: sprites , 2001, Cognition.
[40] D. Knill,et al. The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[41] Alison J. Wiggett,et al. Patterns of fMRI Activity Dissociate Overlapping Functional Brain Areas that Respond to Biological Motion , 2006, Neuron.
[42] A. Rider,et al. Motion-induced position shifts in global dynamic Gabor arrays. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[43] Markus Lappe,et al. Local-to-global form interference in biological motion perception , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[44] D. Sheinberg,et al. Temporal Cortex Neurons Encode Articulated Actions as Slow Sequences of Integrated Poses , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[45] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[46] Maggie Shiffrar,et al. The perception of biological motion across apertures , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[47] Markus Lappe,et al. Brain activity for peripheral biological motion in the posterior superior temporal gyrus and the fusiform gyrus: Dependence on visual hemifield and view orientation , 2009, NeuroImage.
[48] D. Perrett,et al. Integration of form and motion in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area (STPa) of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[49] Wendy Baccus,et al. Form and motion make independent contributions to the response to biological motion in occipitotemporal cortex , 2012, NeuroImage.
[50] Zhong-Lin Lu,et al. A First- and Second-Order Motion Energy Analysis of Peripheral Motion Illusions Leads to Further Evidence of “Feature Blur” in Peripheral Vision , 2011, PloS one.
[51] J. Lange,et al. Visual perception of biological motion by form: a template-matching analysis. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[52] Dorita H. F. Chang,et al. Acceleration carries the local inversion effect in biological motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[53] F. Simion,et al. A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[54] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. Stimulus magnification equates identification and discrimination of biological motion across the visual field , 2008, Vision Research.
[55] D. Burr,et al. The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.
[56] Eric Hiris,et al. Temporal properties in masking biological motion , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[57] Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al. Global Processing of Biological Motions , 1994 .
[58] Randolph Blake,et al. Eccentric perception of biological motion is unscalably poor , 2005, Vision Research.
[59] M. Thirkettle,et al. Contributions of form, motion and task to biological motion perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[60] P. Sinha,et al. Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[61] Alan L. F. Lee,et al. A comparison of global motion perception using a multiple-aperture stimulus. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[62] R. Blake,et al. Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[63] Amelia R Hunt,et al. Disorganizing biological motion. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[64] J A Beintema,et al. Perception of biological motion without local image motion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[65] Chang-Bing Huang,et al. Transitions between Central and Peripheral Vision Create Spatial/Temporal Distortions: A Hypothesis Concerning the Perceived Break of the Curveball , 2010, PloS one.
[66] Nikolaus F. Troje,et al. The local inversion effect in biological motion perception is acceleration-based , 2010 .
[67] Steven M. Thurman,et al. Perceptual and computational analysis of critical features for biological motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[68] Harold E. Bedell,et al. Spatial and temporal properties of the illusory motion-induced position shift for drifting stimuli , 2007, Vision Research.
[69] Z. Kourtzi,et al. Linking form and motion in the primate brain , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[70] Giorgio Vallortigara,et al. Gravity bias in the interpretation of biological motion by inexperienced chicks , 2006, Current Biology.
[71] Zili Liu,et al. Computing dynamic classification images from correlation maps. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[72] P.-J. Hsieh,et al. The infinite regress illusion reveals faulty integration of local and global motion signals , 2006, Vision Research.
[73] Dennis M. Levi,et al. PII: 0042-6989(95)00264-2 , 1997 .