Irrational choices in hummingbird foraging behaviour

It is conventionally assumed that, when animals evaluate alternative options, the value assigned to an option is absolute and independent of the other options available. It follows that animal choices should exhibit the rational property of regularity whereby the proportion of choices for an option cannot be increased by the addition of further options to the choice set. However, violations of regularity occur in human decision making, suggesting that humans may use comparative evaluation mechanisms whereby the value of an option is computed relative to the other options available. For example, in the asymmetrically dominated decoy effect the preference for a target option over a competitor is altered by the addition of a decoy option that is inferior to the target and competitor on one attribute, but lies between them on a second. We tested whether foraging wild rufous hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus, would demonstrate violations of regularity in response to an asymmetrically dominated decoy. Sixteen birds chose between three artificial flower types (Target: 15 μl, 40% sucrose; Competitor: 45 μl, 30%; Decoy: 10 μl, 35%) in Binary (Target versus Competitor) and Trinary (Target versus Competitor versus Decoy) treatments. We predicted higher preference for the Target in the Trinary treatment. The birds ranked the three options in the same order in the Binary and Trinary treatments (Competitor>Target>Decoy). Seven birds showed violations of regularity, six increasing their absolute preference for the Competitor in the Trinary treatment. Overall, relative preference for the Competitor over the Target was higher in the Trinary than in the Binary treatment. These changes in preference are incompatible with an absolute evaluation mechanism.

[1]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Individual Choice Behavior , 1959 .

[2]  A. Tversky Intransitivity of preferences. , 1969 .

[3]  Donald L. Rumelhart,et al.  Similarity between stimuli: An experimental test of the Luce and Restle choice models. , 1971 .

[4]  E Fantino,et al.  Transitivity as a property of choice. , 1972, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[5]  R. Luce,et al.  The Choice Axiom after Twenty Years , 1977 .

[6]  Christopher P. Puto,et al.  Adding Asymmetrically Dominated Alternatives: Violations of Regularity & the Similarity Hypothesis. , 1981 .

[7]  M. Andersson Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird , 1982, Nature.

[8]  Joel Huber,et al.  Market Boundaries and Product Choice: Illustrating Attraction and Substitution Effects , 1983 .

[9]  J. E. Mazur Tests of an equivalence rule for fixed and variable reinforcer delays. , 1984 .

[10]  A. Kacelnik CENTRAL PLACE FORAGING IN STARLINGS (STURNUS-VULGARIS) .1. PATCH RESIDENCE TIME , 1984 .

[11]  C. L. Gass,et al.  Specialization by territorial hummingbirds on experimentally enriched patches of flowers: energetic profitability and learning , 1985 .

[12]  A. Houston,et al.  Partial preferences and foraging , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  S. Tamm Importance of Energy Costs in Central Place Foraging by Hummingbirds , 1989 .

[14]  T. Caraco,et al.  Risk-sensitivity: ambient temperature affects foraging choice , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  Juan C. Reboreda,et al.  Risk sensitivity in starlings: variability in food amount and food delay , 1991 .

[16]  M. Petrie,et al.  Peahens prefer peacocks with elaborate trains , 1991, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  D. H. Wedell,et al.  Distinguishing Among Models of Contextually Induced Preference Reversals , 1991 .

[18]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The Advantage Model: A Comparative Theory of Evaluation and Choice under Risk , 1993 .

[19]  A. Tversky,et al.  Context-dependent preferences , 1993 .

[20]  S. Shafir Intransitivity of preferences in honey bees: support for 'comparative' evaluation of foraging options , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[21]  R. Gibson Female choice in sage grouse: the roles of attraction and active comparison , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[22]  Melissa Bateson,et al.  The energetic costs of alternative rate currencies in the foraging starling , 1996 .

[23]  Alex Kacelnik,et al.  Rate currencies and the foraging starling: the fallacy of the averages revisited , 1996 .

[24]  S. Collins,et al.  Does beak colour affect female preference in zebra finches? , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  L. Real Paradox, Performance, and the Architecture of Decision-Making in Animals' , 1996 .

[26]  M. W. Roberts Hummingbirds' nectar concentration preferences at low volume: the importance of time scale , 1996, Animal Behaviour.

[27]  Alasdair I. Houston,et al.  Natural selection and context-dependent values , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[28]  J. Doyle,et al.  The robustness of the asymmetrically dominated effect: Buying frames, phantom alternatives, and in‐store purchases , 1999 .

[29]  Dan Ariely,et al.  Contextual and procedural determinants of partner selection : Of asymmetric dominance and prominence , 1999 .

[30]  T. Andrew Hurly,et al.  Context-dependent, risk-sensitive foraging preferences in wild rufous hummingbirds , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Energy budgets and risk-sensitive foraging in starlings , 1999 .

[32]  I. Mclaren,et al.  Tests of the Ratio Rule in Categorization , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[33]  Thomas A. Waite,et al.  Background context and decision making in hoarding gray jays , 2001 .

[34]  T. Waite Intransitive preferences in hoarding gray jays (Perisoreus canadensis) , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.