Feedback functions for variable-interval reinforcement.

On a given variable-interval schedule, the average obtained rate of reinforcement depends on the average rate of responding. An expression for this feedback effect is derived from the assumptions that free-operant responding occurs in bursts with a constant tempo, alternating with periods of engagement in other activities; that the durations of bursts and other activities are exponentially distributed; and that the rates of initiating and terminating bursts are inversely related. The expression provides a satisfactory account of the data of three experiments.

[1]  W M Baum,et al.  Optimization and the matching law as accounts of instrumental behavior. , 1981, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[2]  W M Baum,et al.  Time allocation in human vigilance. , 1975, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[3]  T. F. Gilbert Fundamental dimensional properties of the operant. , 1958, Psychological review.

[4]  R. Herrnstein On the law of effect. , 1970, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[5]  F. Mcsweeney Concurrent schedule responding as a function of body weight , 1975 .

[6]  H. Rachlin,et al.  Effects of alternative reinforcement: does the source matter? , 1972, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[7]  R J Herrnstein,et al.  Formal properties of the matching law. , 1974, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[8]  J J Pear,et al.  Constituents of response rates. , 1979, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[9]  D. Stubbs,et al.  Concurrent responding with fixed relative rate of reinforcement. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[10]  H. Rachlin A molar theory of reinforcement schedules. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[11]  R. Herrnstein,et al.  Toward a law of response strength. , 1976 .

[12]  A molecular analysis of multiple schedule interactions: negative contrast. , 1977, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[13]  J. Staddon,et al.  On matching and maximizing in operant choice experiments. , 1978 .

[14]  L R Gollub,et al.  Behavioral interactions in multiple variable-interval schedules. , 1974, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[15]  David Premack,et al.  Distributional properties of operant-level locomotion in the rat. , 1962, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[16]  J. Staddon Operant behavior as adaptation to constraint. , 1979 .

[17]  W M Baum,et al.  The correlation-based law of effect. , 1973, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[18]  D S Blough,et al.  Interresponse time as a function of continuous variables: a new method and some data. , 1963, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[19]  W. Baum,et al.  Time-based and count-based measurement of preference. , 1976, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[20]  William M. Baum,et al.  CHAINED CONCURRENT SCHEDULES: REINFORCEMENT AS SITUATION TRANSITION1 , 1974 .

[21]  G M Heyman,et al.  A Markov model description of changeover probabilities on concurrent variable-interval schedules. , 1979, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[22]  Drazen Prelec,et al.  Feedback functions for reinforcement: A paradigmatic experiment , 1978 .

[23]  Ronald E. Schaub Analyses of Interresponse Times with Small Class Intervals , 1967 .

[24]  W M Baum,et al.  Time allocation and negative reinforcement. , 1973, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[25]  R. Luce,et al.  Operant matching is not a logical consequence of maximizing reinforcement rate , 1979 .