Receptive fields for smooth pursuit eye movements and motion perception

Humans use smooth pursuit eye movements to track moving objects of interest. In order to track an object accurately, motion signals from the target have to be integrated and segmented from motion signals in the visual context. Most studies on pursuit eye movements used small visual targets against a featureless background, disregarding the requirements of our natural visual environment. Here, we tested the ability of the pursuit and the perceptual system to integrate motion signals across larger areas of the visual field. Stimuli were random-dot kinematograms containing a horizontal motion signal, which was perturbed by a spatially localized, peripheral motion signal. Perturbations appeared in a gaze-contingent coordinate system and had a different direction than the main motion including a vertical component. We measured pursuit and perceptual direction discrimination decisions and found that both steady-state pursuit and perception were influenced most by perturbation angles close to that of the main motion signal and only in regions close to the center of gaze. The narrow direction bandwidth (26 angular degrees full width at half height) and small spatial extent (8 degrees of visual angle standard deviation) correspond closely to tuning parameters of neurons in the middle temporal area (MT).

[1]  G H Recanzone,et al.  Shift in smooth pursuit initiation and MT and MST neuronal activity under different stimulus conditions. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[3]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Contextual effects on motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements , 2008, Brain Research.

[4]  Heinen,et al.  PII: S0042-6989(97)00422-7 , 1998 .

[5]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—I. Periodic target steps , 1979, Vision Research.

[6]  J. Movshon,et al.  Dynamics of motion signaling by neurons in macaque area MT , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Receptive-field properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area (MT) of owl monkeys. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Dynamic properties of neurons in cortical area MT in alert and anaesthetized macaque monkeys , 2001, Nature.

[9]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[10]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Processing of retinal and extraretinal signals for memory-guided saccades during smooth pursuit. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Perceived speed of moving lines depends on orientation, length, speed and luminance , 1993, Vision Research.

[13]  Guillaume S Masson,et al.  Object motion computation for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in humans. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Jeroen B. J. Smeets,et al.  Prediction of saccadic amplitude during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2000 .

[15]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Contrast and assimilation in motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Vector Averaging for Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements Initiated by Two Moving Targets in Monkeys , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Uwe J. Ilg,et al.  Cancellation of self-induced retinal image motion during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2001, Vision Research.

[18]  Christopher C. Pack,et al.  Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.

[19]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Distractor interference during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Neuronal responses in visual areas MT and MST during smooth pursuit target selection. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  L S Stone,et al.  Motion coherence affects human perception and pursuit similarly , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.

[22]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—II. Single target displacements , 1979, Vision Research.

[23]  Brian H Scott,et al.  A comparison of pursuit eye movement and perceptual performance in speed discrimination. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[24]  Brent R. Beutter,et al.  Human motion perception and smooth eye movements slow similar directional biases for elongated apertures , 1998, Vision Research.

[25]  L. Stone,et al.  From following edges to pursuing objects. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  W T Newsome,et al.  How Is a Sensory Map Read Out? Effects of Microstimulation in Visual Area MT on Saccades and Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Stephen J Heinen,et al.  Human smooth pursuit direction discrimination , 1999, Vision Research.

[28]  R. Kanai,et al.  Inhibition of saccade initiation by preceding smooth pursuit , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Luc Proteau,et al.  Effects of stationary and moving textured backgrounds on the visuo-oculo-manual tracking in humans , 1995, Vision Research.

[30]  P. van Donkelaar,et al.  The allocation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[31]  Maninder K. Kahlon,et al.  Visual Motion Analysis for Pursuit Eye Movements in Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Guillaume S Masson,et al.  Temporal dynamics of 2D motion integration for ocular following in macaque monkeys. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Aya Takemura,et al.  Ocular tracking of moving targets: effects of perturbing the background. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Richard J. Krauzlis,et al.  Spatial allocation of attention during smooth pursuit eye movements , 2009, Vision Research.

[35]  Eric Castet,et al.  Parallel Motion Processing for the Initiation of Short-Latency Ocular Following in Humans , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Direction Perception in Complex Dynamic Displays: the Integration of Dir~~tion Information , 1988 .

[37]  T. Albright Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Latency of saccades during smooth-pursuit eye movement in man Directional asymmetries , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Shared attentional control of smooth eye movement and perception , 1986, Vision Research.

[40]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Contextual effects on smooth-pursuit eye movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Different motion sensitive units are involved in recovering the direction of moving lines , 1993, Vision Research.

[42]  K Kawano,et al.  Ocular responses to brief motion of textured backgrounds during smooth pursuit in humans. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.