Influence of delayed timing of owners' actions on the behaviors of their dogs, Canis familiaris
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Ludwig Huber,et al. Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their owners. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.
[2] W. J. Griffiths,et al. Free-Operant Acquisition with Delayed Reinforcement , 1992 .
[3] Á. Miklósi,et al. Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs , 1998, Animal Cognition.
[4] K. Lattal,et al. Response acquisition with delayed reinforcement. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[5] L. Coppinger,et al. Observations on assistance dog training and use. , 1998, Journal of applied animal welfare science : JAAWS.
[6] József Topál,et al. Dogs' (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.
[7] A. Baron,et al. Density and delay of punishment of free-operant avoidance. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[8] N A Neef,et al. Assessing influential dimensions of reinforcers on choice in students with serious emotional disturbance. , 1994, Journal of applied behavior analysis.
[9] G. Ellison,et al. DIFFERENTIAL SALIVARY CONDITIONING TO TRACES. , 1964, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology.
[10] D. Mills,et al. More than just a word: non-semantic command variables affect obedience in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) , 2005 .
[11] Satoshi Yamazaki,et al. Application of Implicit Learning to Measurement of Psychological Effect of Transmission Delay in Audiovisual Communication , 2002 .
[12] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic Dogs (Canis familiaris) Use Human and Conspecific Social Cues to Locate Hidden Food , 1999 .
[13] T. Critchfield,et al. Acquisition of a spatially defined operant with delayed reinforcement. , 1993, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[14] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food , 2005, Animal Cognition.
[15] Á. Miklósi,et al. Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus , 2004, Behavioural Processes.
[16] Clifford Nass,et al. The media equation - how people treat computers, television, and new media like real people and places , 1996 .
[17] N. Schneiderman. Interstimulus interval function of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit under delay versus trace conditioning. , 1966 .
[18] T Purryag,et al. Culture clash. , 1995, Nursing standard (Royal College of Nursing (Great Britain) : 1987).
[19] E. Depiereux,et al. Training methods of military dog handlers and their effects on the team's performances , 2008 .
[20] H. Schlinger,et al. The Effects of Delayed Reinforcement and a Response-Produced Auditory Stimulus on the Acquisition of Operant Behavior in Rats , 1994 .
[21] N. Rooney,et al. Do dogs respond to play signals given by humans? , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[22] N. Braveman,et al. The combined effects of dosage level and interstimulus interval on the formation of one-trial poison-based aversions in rats , 1975 .
[23] R. Solomon,et al. Some effects of delay of punishment on resistance to temptation in dogs. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[24] M. Tomasello,et al. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[25] M. Tomasello,et al. Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use , 2000, Animal Cognition.
[26] Thomas D. Sambrook,et al. Use of human-given cues by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and horses (Equus caballus) , 2000, Animal Cognition.