Spatial updating of locations specified by 3-d sound and spatial language.

Blind and blindfolded sighted observers were presented with auditory stimuli specifying target locations. The stimulus was either sound from a loudspeaker or spatial language (e.g., "2 o'clock, 16 ft"). On each trial, an observer attempted to walk to the target location along a direct or indirect path. The ability to mentally keep track of the target location without concurrent perceptual information about it (spatial updating) was assessed in terms of the separation between the stopping points for the 2 paths. Updating performance was very nearly the same for the 2 modalities, indicating that once an internal representation of a location has been determined, subsequent updating performance is nearly independent of the modality used to specify the representation.

[1]  C C Presson,et al.  Updating after Rotational and Translational Body Movements: Coordinate Structure of Perspective Space , 1994, Perception.

[2]  Michael Tlauka,et al.  Orientation specificity occurs in both small- and large-scale imagined routes presented as verbal descriptions. , 1999 .

[3]  P.O. Barber,et al.  Encoding Direction in Manipulatory Space and the Role of Visual Experience , 1988 .

[4]  J. Thomson Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  The coding and transformation of spatial information , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  M Behrmann,et al.  Active control of locomotion facilitates nonvisual navigation. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  M. Hegarty,et al.  Properties of cognitive maps constructed from texts , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[8]  T. Gärling,et al.  Maintenance of orientation during locomotion in unfamiliar environments. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  GPS-Based Navigation Systems for the Visually Impaired , 2001 .

[10]  J. Rieser,et al.  Sensitivity to Perspective Structure While Walking without Vision , 1986, Perception.

[11]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. , 2003 .

[12]  Yuichi Ohta,et al.  Mixed Reality: Merging Real and Virtual Worlds , 1999 .

[13]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Auditory Distance Perception in Real, Virtual, and Mixed Environments , 1999 .

[14]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Auditory distance perception by translating observers , 1993, Proceedings of 1993 IEEE Research Properties in Virtual Reality Symposium.

[15]  Susanna Millar Understanding and representing space , 1994 .

[16]  Walter Kintsch,et al.  Propositional and situational representations of text , 1985 .

[17]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  J. Rieser,et al.  Young Children's Spatial Orientation with Respect to Multiple Targets When Walking without Vision. , 1991 .

[19]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Perceiving Real-World Viewpoint Changes , 1998 .

[20]  Mark Holllns,et al.  Spatial updating in blind and sighted people , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Alain Savoyant,et al.  Chapter 2 Visual imagery in locomotor movement without vision , 1991 .

[22]  Martin J. Farrell,et al.  Mental Rotation and the Automatic Updating of Body-Centered Spatial Relationships , 1998 .

[23]  J. Rieser Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[25]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Performance of Blind and Sighted Persons on Spatial Tasks , 1995 .

[26]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[27]  Woodrow Barfield,et al.  Fundamentals of Wearable Computers and Augumented Reality , 2000 .

[28]  Robert H. Logie,et al.  Mental images in human cognition , 1991 .

[29]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense? , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  A Geographical Information System for a GPS Based Personal Guidance System , 1998, Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Sci..

[31]  S S Fukusima,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  J. Patrick The effect of interpolated motor activities in short-term motor memory. , 1971, Journal of motor behavior.

[33]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Assessing auditory distance perception using perceptually directed action , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  J M Loomis,et al.  Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[35]  A Berthoz,et al.  Updating an object’s orientation and location during nonvisual navigation: A comparison between two processing modes , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[36]  James A. Thomson,et al.  On-Line Updating of Spatial Information Druing Locomotion Without Vision. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[37]  Holly A. Taylorandbarbaratversky Spatial Mental Models Derived from Survey and Route Descriptions , 1992 .

[38]  S. Millar Understanding and Representing Space: Theory and Evidence from Studies with Blind and Sighted Children , 1994 .

[39]  D H Ashmead,et al.  Contribution of listeners' approaching motion to auditory distance perception. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  R. D. Easton,et al.  Object-array structure, frames of reference, and retrieval of spatial knowledge. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.