A possible role for primary motor cortex during action observation

Social interaction depends on the ability to infer beliefs and intentions in the minds of others. Little is known about the neural basis of our ability to “read” the intentions of others, but a likely candidate is the mirror-neuron system (MNS). Mirror neurons discharge not only during action execution but also during action observation. It is this property that makes these neurons a possible neural substrate for action understanding. The notion that actions are intrinsically linked to perception was proposed by William James, who suggested that “every mental representation of a movement awakens to some degree the actual movement which is its object” (1). The implication is that observing, imagining, preparing, or in any way representing an action excites the motor program used to execute that same action (2, 3).

[1]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[2]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[3]  Y. Paulignan,et al.  An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.

[4]  R. Lemon,et al.  Macaque ventral premotor cortex exerts powerful facilitation of motor cortex outputs to upper limb motoneurons. , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  J. Decety,et al.  Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[6]  E. Pucci,et al.  Percutaneous ethanol injection therapy of autonomous nodule and amiodarone-induced thyrotoxicosis. , 1994, Thyroidology.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[9]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Afferent and efferent projections of the inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[10]  H. Jasper,et al.  Electrocorticograms in man: Effect of voluntary movement upon the electrical activity of the precentral gyrus , 1949 .

[11]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  R. Hari,et al.  Spatiotemporal characteristics of sensorimotor neuromagnetic rhythms related to thumb movement , 1994, Neuroscience.

[13]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Actor's and observer's primary motor cortices stabilize similarly after seen or heard motor actions , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[15]  D. Hoffman,et al.  Direction of action is represented in the ventral premotor cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons , 2002, Science.

[17]  D. Hoffman,et al.  Sensorimotor transformations in cortical motor areas , 2003, Neuroscience Research.

[18]  P. Strick,et al.  Frontal Lobe Inputs to the Digit Representations of the Motor Areas on the Lateral Surface of the Hemisphere , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. , 2007, Neuroreport.

[20]  H. Gastaut [Electrocorticographic study of the reactivity of rolandic rhythm]. , 1952, Revue neurologique.

[21]  D. Perrett,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.11 November 2004 Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective , 2022 .

[22]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  Eiji Hoshi,et al.  Movement-related neuronal activity reflecting the transformation of coordinates in the ventral premotor cortex of monkeys. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  W. N. Schoenfeld,et al.  Principles of Psychology , 2007 .

[25]  E. Fetz,et al.  Coherent 25- to 35-Hz oscillations in the sensorimotor cortex of awake behaving monkeys. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  S. Cochin,et al.  Observation and execution of movement: similarities demonstrated by quantified electroencephalography , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[27]  E. Procyk,et al.  Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.