Editorial: Improving visual deficits with perceptual learning

The capability of improving performance on visual tasks with practice has been a matter of intense investigation during the last 40 years (Fiorentini and Berardi, 1980; Sagi, 2011). This phenomenon, called perceptual learning, has been proven to occur with virtually any visual skill or stimulus characteristic (Fahle and Poggio, 2002), and to be long-lasting, thus involving neural plasticity at the level of perceptual or even sensory areas (Sagi and Tanne, 1994). Despite this, only recently has perceptual learning started to be considered a useful tool for improving visual functions in clinical populations. This delayed exploitation has possibly been caused by the common finding that learning was highly specific for the trained stimulus attributes (Fiorentini and Berardi, 1980; Ball and Sekuler, 1981; Ahissar and Hochstein, 1996; Schoups et al., 2001; Campana and Casco, 2003; Fahle, 2005), or even for the trained eye or retinal location (Karni and Sagi, 1991), thus resulting impractical for therapeutic purposes. More recently it has become clear that, under specific training conditions, perceptual learning could generalize to other stimuli, tasks and circumstances (McGovern et al., 2012), yielding potential benefits for various types of visual impairments. So far, perceptual learning has been shown to be effective in improving, among other dysfunctions, visual abilities in amblyopia (Levi and Li, 2009; Polat, 2009; Hussain et al., 2012), mild refractive defects (myopia: Tan and Fong, 2008; Camilleri et al., 2014a; presbyopia: Polat et al., 2012), central or peripheral vision loss and cortical blindness (Kasten et al., 1998; Sabel et al., 2005; Huxlin et al., 2009; Chung, 2011; Das et al., 2014), dyslexia (Gori and Facoetti, 2015), and has even been shown to improve the efficacy of other sensory modalities so that they can somehow replace vision (so called sensory substitution) in blind people (Bach-y-Rita and Kercel, 2003; Ortiz et al., 2011). The goal of this Research Topic is to demonstrate the development of innovative methods, based on perceptual learning, for treating—or at least overcoming some of the deleterious effects of—various visual dysfunctions, from mild deficits such as myopia to complete blindness. New frontier methods should aim at finding the most effective procedures both in terms of perceptual learning and transfer to useful visual functions. This is made possible by combining different techniques aimed at boosting learning or its generalization, such as training with different stimulus features (Xiao et al., 2008; Harris et al., 2012), exploiting multisensory facilitation (Shams and Seitz, 2008) and reinforcement procedures (Seitz and Watanabe, 2009), or combining perceptual learning with non-invasive brain stimulation procedures (Fertonani et al., 2011). Also, in order to achieve the best possible compliance with the patients, shorter and/or more enjoyable trainings (possibly self-administered at home) should be preferred. For example, while training on either off-the shelf video games (Li et al., 2011; Franceschini et al., 2013), or specifically designed video games involving detection of low contrast stimuli (Deveau et al., 2014a,b) has been shown to improve a range of visual functions (visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, reading skills and even sport performances) both in normally sighted people and people with developmental dyslexia or amblyopia, in the present Research Topic we see that the latter type of video games can also improve visual acuity in participants with refractive defects such presbyopia (Deveau and Seitz, 2014) or reduce crowding (the deleterious effect of nearby elements on target's perception; Levi, 2008) in participants with cortical deficits such as amblyopia (Hussain et al., 2014). While negligible in normal foveal vision, crowding is an important issue also in children with visual impairment accompanied by nystagmus. Reduction of crowding in these children (besides an improvement of near visual acuity, see Huurneman et al., 2013) can be obtained with training on crowded letters, thus producing faster reaction times and an increase of fixation durations (Huurneman and Boonstra, 2014). Visual functions in participants with mild refractive defects or amblyopia have also been shown to considerably improve with contrast detection trainings (with or without lateral masking) (Tan and Fong, 2008; Levi and Li, 2009; Polat, 2009; Polat et al., 2012; Camilleri et al., 2014a). Here we see how, both in mild myopia and amblyopia, combining a contrast detection training with non-invasive brain stimulation (specifically, transcranial random noise stimulation—tRNS) seems to yield to faster/more effective perceptual learning and transfer to visual acuity and contrast sensitivity (Camilleri et al., 2014b; Campana et al., 2014). Perceptual learning can also be successfully applied to patients with loss of central vision. Indeed, past research has shown, in sighted participants, how perceptual learning on a contrast detection task with lateral masking was able to reduce crowding at eccentric retinal locations (Maniglia et al., 2011). Here we see how, in patients with macular degeneration, eccentric perceptual learning with a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) produces an improvement in reading speed mainly with supra-threshold word durations (above 200 ms) (Coates and Chung, 2014), while a texture discrimination training enhances temporal processing of eccentric stimuli (reflected in shorter stimulus onset asynchrony needed for discrimination), especially when fixation was stable (Plank et al., 2014). In fact improved temporal processing in areas of residual vision (besides an extension of such areas) in patients with vision loss (hemianopia or quadrantanopia) can be also obtained with the so-called vision restoration therapy, an individualized program providing stimulation at the border of the dysfunctional visual field (Poggel et al., 2014). Finally, perceptual learning could be useful even for blind people. Blindness often produces an impaired spatial representation in other sensory domains (e.g., Gori et al., 2014a). Here it is shown that blindfolded sighted participants can learn an auditory spatial bisection task, but improvements only occur when a tactile feedback is delivered, indicating that the tactile system can be used to recalibrate the spatial representation in the auditory domain (Gori et al., 2014b). This finding suggests that, also in blind people, auditory spatial representation can be improved via tactile feedback. To sum up the findings of the present Research Topic, the studies collected here provide the frontline of behavioral and brain stimulation-coupled treatments of a heterogeneous ensemble of visual dysfunctions. Future studies are needed to define the best combination of approaches in order to improve vision with the shortest and most efficacious training, increasing patients' compliance and tailoring the training specifically for each patients' needs.

[1]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Improved vision and on-field performance in baseball through perceptual learning , 2014, Current Biology.

[2]  H. Strasburger,et al.  A matter of time: improvement of visual temporal processing during training-induced restoration of light detection performance , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[3]  Gianluca Campana,et al.  Learning in combined-feature search: Specificity to orientation , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  D. Scott Perceptual learning. , 1974, Queen's nursing journal.

[5]  M. Greenlee,et al.  Perceptual learning in patients with macular degeneration , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[6]  Uri Polat,et al.  Training the brain to overcome the effect of aging on the human eye , 2012, Scientific Reports.

[7]  Gianluca Campana,et al.  Reducing Crowding by Weakening Inhibitory Lateral Interactions in the Periphery with Perceptual Learning , 2011, PloS one.

[8]  G. Orban,et al.  Practising orientation identification improves orientation coding in V1 neurons , 2001, Nature.

[9]  P. Bach-y-Rita,et al.  Sensory substitution and the human–machine interface , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  D. Sagi,et al.  Generalized Perceptual Learning in the Absence of Sensory Adaptation , 2012, Current Biology.

[11]  D. Tanné,et al.  Perceptual learning: learning to see , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Learning Pop-out Detection: Specificities to Stimulus Characteristics , 1996, Vision Research.

[13]  S. Klein,et al.  Complete Transfer of Perceptual Learning across Retinal Locations Enabled by Double Training , 2008, Current Biology.

[14]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Benefits of multisensory learning , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Paul V. McGraw,et al.  The challenges of developing a contrast-based video game for treatment of amblyopia , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[16]  F. Boonstra,et al.  Training shortens search times in children with visual impairment accompanied by nystagmus , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[17]  Duje Tadin,et al.  Beyond Blindsight: Properties of Visual Relearning in Cortically Blind Fields , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Daniel R. Coates,et al.  Changes across the psychometric function following perceptual learning of an RSVP reading task , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[19]  G. V. van Rens,et al.  Perceptual learning in children with visual impairment improves near visual acuity. , 2013, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[20]  G. Campana,et al.  Improving visual functions in adult amblyopia with combined perceptual training and transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS): a pilot study , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[21]  M. Fahle Perceptual learning: specificity versus generalization , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[22]  A. Fiorentini,et al.  Perceptual learning specific for orientation and spatial frequency , 1980, Nature.

[23]  Paul V McGraw,et al.  Perceptual Learning Reduces Crowding in Amblyopia and in the Normal Periphery , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Mary Hayhoe,et al.  Perceptual Relearning of Complex Visual Motion after V1 Damage in Humans , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Jonathan W Peirce,et al.  Transfer of perceptual learning between different visual tasks. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[26]  S. Gori,et al.  Action Video Games Make Dyslexic Children Read Better , 2013, Current Biology.

[27]  Uri Polat,et al.  Making perceptual learning practical to improve visual functions , 2009, Vision Research.

[28]  Gianluca Campana,et al.  Improving myopia via perceptual learning: is training with lateral masking the only (or the most) efficacious technique? , 2014, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Recruitment of Occipital Cortex during Sensory Substitution Training Linked to Subjective Experience of Seeing in People with Blindness , 2011, PloS one.

[30]  Roger W. Li,et al.  Video-Game Play Induces Plasticity in the Visual System of Adults with Amblyopia , 2011, PLoS biology.

[31]  D Sagi,et al.  Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  C. Miniussi,et al.  Random Noise Stimulation Improves Neuroplasticity in Perceptual Learning , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Perceptual learning as a potential treatment for amblyopia: A mini-review , 2009, Vision Research.

[34]  N. Newman,et al.  Vision restoration therapy , 2005, British Journal of Ophthalmology.

[35]  Bernhard A. Sabel,et al.  Computer-based training for the treatment of partial blindness , 1998, Nature Medicine.

[36]  S. Gori,et al.  How the visual aspects can be crucial in reading acquisition? The intriguing case of crowding and developmental dyslexia. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[37]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  The phenomenon of task-irrelevant perceptual learning , 2009, Vision Research.

[38]  G. Campana,et al.  Improvement of uncorrected visual acuity and contrast sensitivity with perceptual learning and transcranial random noise stimulation in individuals with mild myopia , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[39]  Susana T. L. Chung Improving reading speed for people with central vision loss through perceptual learning. , 2011, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[40]  R Sekuler,et al.  Adaptive processing of visual motion. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Broad-based visual benefits from training with an integrated perceptual-learning video game , 2014, Vision Research.

[42]  Monica Gori,et al.  Tactile feedback improves auditory spatial localization , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[43]  G. Sandini,et al.  Impairment of auditory spatial localization in congenitally blind human subjects , 2013, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[44]  D. Sagi Perceptual learning in Vision Research , 2011, Vision Research.

[45]  D. Levi Crowding—An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review , 2008, Vision Research.

[46]  D. Tan,et al.  Efficacy of neural vision therapy to enhance contrast sensitivity function and visual acuity in low myopia , 2008, Journal of Cataract & Refractive Surgery.

[47]  G. Orban,et al.  Practising orientation identi ® cation improves orientation coding in V 1 neurons , 2022 .

[48]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Applying perceptual learning to achieve practical changes in vision , 2014, Front. Psychol..