The Effect of Task on Object Processing revealed by EEG decoding
暂无分享,去创建一个
Yetta Kwailing Wong | Hoi Ming Ken Yip | Leo Y. T. Cheung | Alan C.-N. Wong | A. Wong | Vince S H Ngan | Y. Wong | Hoi Ming Ken Yip
[1] J. Haynes. A Primer on Pattern-Based Approaches to fMRI: Principles, Pitfalls, and Perspectives , 2015, Neuron.
[2] R. Itier,et al. Effects of task demands on the early neural processing of fearful and happy facial expressions , 2017, Brain Research.
[3] Radoslaw Martin Cichy,et al. The representational dynamics of task and object processing in humans , 2018, eLife.
[4] B. Scholl,et al. Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for “top-down” effects , 2015, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[5] Nicole M. Long,et al. Bottom-Up and Top-Down Factors Differentially Influence Stimulus Representations Across Large-Scale Attentional Networks , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[6] C. Teufel,et al. How to (and how not to) think about top-down influences on visual perception , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[7] Berenice Valdés-Conroy,et al. Following the time course of face gender and expression processing: a task-dependent ERP study. , 2014, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[8] Simon van Gaal,et al. No Evidence that Predictions and Attention Modulate the First Feedforward Sweep of Cortical Information Processing , 2018, bioRxiv.
[9] J. Hohwy. Priors in perception: Top-down modulation, Bayesian perceptual learning rate, and prediction error minimization , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[10] Geraint Rees,et al. Pattern classification using functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[11] Jeffrey S. Johnson,et al. The time course of encoding and maintenance of task-relevant versus irrelevant object features in working memory , 2019, Cortex.
[12] Isabel Gauthier,et al. An early electrophysiological response associated with expertise in letter perception , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[13] J. Hohwy. The Predictive Mind , 2013 .
[14] Karl J. Friston. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[15] Thomas Serre,et al. A feedforward architecture accounts for rapid categorization , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[16] Thomas Serre,et al. A Theory of Object Recognition: Computations and Circuits in the Feedforward Path of the Ventral Stream in Primate Visual Cortex , 2005 .
[17] Keiji Tanaka,et al. Matching Categorical Object Representations in Inferior Temporal Cortex of Man and Monkey , 2008, Neuron.
[18] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[19] Na Yeon Kim,et al. Task influences pattern discriminability for faces and bodies in ventral occipitotemporal cortex , 2016, Social neuroscience.
[20] Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al. Attention Improves Encoding of Task-Relevant Features in the Human Visual Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] James J. DiCarlo,et al. How Does the Brain Solve Visual Object Recognition? , 2012, Neuron.
[22] Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al. Less Is More: Expectation Sharpens Representations in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.
[23] Yaoda Xu,et al. Goal-Directed Visual Processing Differentially Impacts Human Ventral and Dorsal Visual Representations , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] Steven J. Luck,et al. ERPLAB: an open-source toolbox for the analysis of event-related potentials , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[25] H. P. Op de Beeck,et al. Task Context Overrules Object- and Category-Related Representational Content in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2017, Cerebral cortex.
[26] Anina N. Rich,et al. Feature-selective Attention in Frontoparietal Cortex: Multivoxel Codes Adjust to Prioritize Task-relevant Information , 2017, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[27] James M. Shine,et al. Predictions penetrate perception: Converging insights from brain, behaviour and disorder , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[28] Dimitrios Pantazis,et al. Multivariate pattern analysis of MEG and EEG: A comparison of representational structure in time and space , 2016, NeuroImage.
[29] Susan G. Wardle,et al. Decoding Dynamic Brain Patterns from Evoked Responses: A Tutorial on Multivariate Pattern Analysis Applied to Time Series Neuroimaging Data , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[30] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[31] T. Allison,et al. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[32] A. Newen,et al. Why cognitive penetration of our perceptual experience is still the most plausible account , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[33] M. Tarr,et al. Becoming a “Greeble” Expert: Exploring Mechanisms for Face Recognition , 1997, Vision Research.
[34] T. Poggio,et al. Neural mechanisms of object recognition , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[35] Chih-Jen Lin,et al. LIBSVM: A library for support vector machines , 2011, TIST.
[36] D. Stokes,et al. Noise, uncertainty, and interest: Predictive coding and cognitive penetration , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[37] Ann-Christine Ehlis,et al. Source Localization of Early Stages of Face Processing , 2005, Brain Topography.
[38] Bruno Rossion,et al. ERP evidence for task modulations on face perceptual processing at different spatial scales , 2003, Cogn. Sci..
[39] A. Raftopoulos. The cognitive impenetrability of the content of early vision is a necessary and sufficient condition for purely nonconceptual content , 2014 .
[40] Gabriel Curio,et al. MEG/EEG sources of the 170-ms response to faces are co-localized in the fusiform gyrus , 2007, NeuroImage.
[41] A. Clark. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[42] J. Tanaka,et al. A Neural Basis for Expert Object Recognition , 2001, Psychological science.
[43] Z. Pylyshyn. Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. , 1999, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[44] S. Fazelpour,et al. Attention in the predictive mind , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[45] A. Burton,et al. Human brain potential correlates of repetition priming in face and name recognition , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[46] Thomas E. Nichols,et al. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[47] Gary Lupyan,et al. Cognitive Penetrability of Perception in the Age of Prediction: Predictive Systems are Penetrable Systems , 2015 .
[48] Russell A. Epstein,et al. Coding of Object Size and Object Category in Human Visual Cortex , 2016, Cerebral cortex.
[49] Kathrin Cohen Kadosh,et al. Task-dependent Activation of Face-sensitive Cortex: An fMRI Adaptation Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[50] N. Kanwisher,et al. The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition , 2001, Vision Research.
[51] Dwight J. Kravitz,et al. Task context impacts visual object processing differentially across the cortex , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[52] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[53] F. Macpherson. The relationship between cognitive penetration and predictive coding , 2017, Consciousness and Cognition.
[54] A. Burton,et al. N250r: a face-selective brain response to stimulus repetitions , 2004, Neuroreport.
[55] James W. Tanaka,et al. A Reevaluation of the Electrophysiological Correlates of Expert Object Processing , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.