Grammatical and nongrammatical contributions to closed-class word selection.

Closed-class word selection was investigated by focusing on determiner production. Native speakers from three different languages named pictures of objects using determiner plus noun phrases (e.g., in French "la table" [the-sub(feminineA) table], while ignoring distractor determiners printed on the pictures (e.g., "le" [the-sub(masculine)]. The target and distractor expressed either shared or different grammatical and nongrammatical features (gender, number, and definiteness). A gender-facilitation effect was observed and attributed to noun processing. Crucially, across five experiments, distractors that shared a feature with the target determiner never resulted in longer naming latencies than distractors that were more different. These results indicate that activating related candidates is not detrimental for determiner retrieval, suggesting a noncompetitive mechanism of closed-class word selection.

[1]  K. Patterson,et al.  Speak and spell: Dissociations and word-class effects. , 1987 .

[2]  A. Friederici Syntactic and semantic processes in aphasic deficits: The availability of prepositions , 1982, Brain and Language.

[3]  Albert Costa,et al.  The gender congruency effect and the selection of freestanding and bound morphemes: evidence from croatian. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  S. Monsell,et al.  Inhibition of Spoken Word Production by Priming a Semantic Competitor , 1994 .

[5]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  W. Glaser,et al.  The time course of picture-word interference. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Jörg D. Jescheniak,et al.  Gender Priming in Picture Naming: Modality and Baseline Effects , 1999 .

[8]  Matthew Goldrick,et al.  Connectionist Principles in Theories of Speech Production , 2007, The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics.

[9]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Grammatical feature selection in noun phrase production: Evidence from German and Dutch , 2003 .

[10]  Brenda C Rapp,et al.  Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology: What Deficits Reveal About the Human Mind , 2001 .

[11]  A. Caramazza,et al.  When more is less: a counterintuitive effect of distractor frequency in the picture-word interference paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  Linnea C. Ehri,et al.  Do Adjectives and Functors Interfere as Much as Nouns in Naming Pictures , 1977 .

[13]  T Jacobsen,et al.  Effects of Grammatical Gender on Picture and Word Naming: Evidence from German , 1999, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[14]  H. Schriefers Syntactic processes in the production of noun phrases , 1993 .

[15]  M. Coltheart,et al.  The cognitive neuropsychology of language , 1987 .

[16]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies , 2006, Cognition.

[17]  Albert Costa,et al.  Level of categorisation effect: A novel effect in the picture-word interference paradigm , 2003 .

[18]  Jean K. Gordon,et al.  Learning to divide the labor: an account of deficits in light and heavy verb production , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[19]  L. Cohen,et al.  Closed-class words in sentence production: Evidence from a modality-specific dissociation , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[20]  Albert Costa,et al.  On the categorical nature of the semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[21]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  On Naming a Giraffe a Zebra: Picture Naming Errors Across Different Object Categories , 1993 .

[22]  F X Alario,et al.  A set of 400 pictures standardized for French: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, and age of acquisition , 1999, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[23]  Holly P. Branigan,et al.  Proceedings of the 21st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society , 1999 .

[24]  A Caramazza,et al.  The selection of determiners in noun phrase production. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  Harry A. Whitaker,et al.  Studies in neurolinguistics , 1976 .

[26]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Determiner selection in noun phrase production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The production of determiners: evidence from French , 2002, Cognition.

[28]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Closed- and open-class lexical access in agrammatic and fluent aphasics , 1983, Brain and Language.

[29]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The Selection of Grammatical Features in Word Production: The Case of Plural Nouns in German , 2002, Brain and Language.

[30]  David Caplan,et al.  Agrammatism in sentence production without comprehension deficits: Reduced availability of syntactic structures and/or of grammatical morphemes? A case study , 1988, Brain and Language.

[31]  G. Vigliocco,et al.  Grammatical gender effects on cognition: implications for language learning and language use. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[32]  Angela D. Friederici,et al.  Computational dissociation of two vocabulary types: Evidence from aphasia , 1980, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  David P. Vinson,et al.  Semantic similarity and grammatical class in naming actions , 2005, Cognition.

[34]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Semantic competition between hierarchically related words during speech planning , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[35]  Thomas Pechmann,et al.  The time course of recovery for grammatical category information during lexical processing for syntactic construction. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[36]  J. Jescheniak,et al.  Semantic and phonological activation in noun and pronoun production. , 2001, Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory and Cognition.

[37]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Memory and cognition , 1977 .

[38]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The Modality-Specific Organization of Grammatical Categories: Evidence from Impaired Spoken and Written Sentence Production , 1997, Brain and Language.

[39]  Albert Costa,et al.  Different selection principles of freestanding and bound morphemes in language production. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[40]  David P Vinson,et al.  Role of grammatical gender and semantics in German word production. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[41]  Albert Costa,et al.  The Gender Congruity Effect: Evidence from Spanish and Catalan , 1999 .

[42]  Edgar Zurif,et al.  Bee but not be: Oral reading of single words in aphasia and alexia , 1975, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Albert Costa,et al.  The absence of a gender congruency effect in romance languages: a matter of stimulus onset asynchrony? , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[44]  A. Roelofs,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.

[45]  W. Levelt,et al.  Semantic and syntactic forces in noun phrase production. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  Harold Goodglass,et al.  Contrasting cases of Italian agrammatic aphasia without comprehension disorder , 1983, Brain and Language.

[47]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Selection of gender-marked morphemes in speech production. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[48]  H. Schriefers,et al.  Grammatical gender in noun phrase production: the gender interference effect in German. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  A. W. Ellis Normality and pathology in cognitive functions , 1982 .

[50]  W. Levelt,et al.  Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words , 2001, Cognition.

[51]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  The processing of free and bound gender-marked morphemes in speech production: evidence from Dutch. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  Albert Costa,et al.  The semantic interference effect in the picture-word interference paradigm: does the response set matter? , 2000, Cognition.

[53]  Kathryn Bock,et al.  Language production : Grammatical encoding , 1994 .

[54]  Herbert Schriefers,et al.  Dominance affects determiner selection in language production , 2005 .

[55]  W. Heij,et al.  Categorical interference and associative priming in picture naming. , 1990 .

[56]  Elizabeth Bates,et al.  Effect of Grammatical Gender and Semantic Context on Lexical Access in Italian , 1999, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[57]  Kenneth I Forster,et al.  DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[58]  G. Assal,et al.  Dissociations in aphasia: A case report , 1981, Brain and Language.

[59]  E. Andreewsky,et al.  Implicit Processing of Grammatical Rules in a Classical Case of Agrammatism , 1975, Cortex.

[60]  Emmanuel Dupoux Language, brain, and cognitive development : essays in honor of Jacques Mehler , 2001 .

[61]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[62]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The selection of closed-class words in noun phrase production: The case of Dutch determiners , 2003 .

[63]  Wido La Heij,et al.  Phonological facilitation of grammatical gender retrieval , 2004 .

[64]  A. Lecours,et al.  Neurolinguistic Analysis of Jargonaphasia and Jargonagraphia**This research has been supported by grant MRC-MA4210. , 1976 .

[65]  D. Howard,et al.  ‘Little words’—not really: function and content words in normal and aphasic speech , 2002, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[66]  Lorella Lotto,et al.  Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture–word interference paradigm , 2005 .

[67]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  Effects of Frequency and Vocabulary Type on Phonological Speech Errors , 1990 .

[68]  Frederick J. Newmeyer Language : psychological and biological aspects , 1988 .

[69]  Albert Costa,et al.  A further look at semantic context effects in language production: The role of response congruency , 2006 .

[70]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The Minimalist Program , 1992 .

[71]  M. F. Garrett,et al.  The Analysis of Sentence Production1 , 1975 .

[72]  Markus F Damian,et al.  Locus of semantic interference in picture-word interference tasks , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[73]  Melanie Vitkovitch,et al.  The Effects of Distractor Words on Naming Pictures at the Subordinate Level , 1999 .

[74]  J. Segui,et al.  Semantic and Associative Priming in Picture Naming , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[75]  Jonathan Grainger,et al.  Psycholinguistique Cognitive: Essais en l'honneur de Juan Segui , 2004 .

[76]  Albert Costa,et al.  Set size and repetition in the picture–word interference paradigm: implications for models of naming , 2001, Cognition.

[77]  Juan Segui,et al.  Gender congruency effects in picture naming. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[78]  A. Lecours,et al.  Biological perspectives on language , 1984 .

[79]  Edeltrud Marx,et al.  Gender Processing in Speech Production: Evidence from German Speech Errors , 1999 .

[80]  M. Garrett Processes in language production , 1988 .

[81]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Where Do Semantic Errors Come From? , 1990, Cortex.

[82]  Stephen J. Lupker,et al.  The semantic nature of response competition in the picture-word interference task , 1979 .

[83]  W. La Heij,et al.  The gender-congruency effect in picture-word tasks , 1998 .